Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
1.
Memory ; 27(9): 1204-1213, 2019 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31304873

RESUMO

Answering questions before learning something ("prequestions") enhances learning. However, these benefits usually occur for information that was asked in the prequestions (i.e. prequestioned material), and not for non-prequestioned material. We reasoned that this narrow benefit may be due to the fact that studies typically use fairly simple prequestions that have a clear answer within one part of the learning material - isolative prequestions. We explored the effects of integrative prequestions that required participants to make connections across different parts of a reading passage. Experiment 1 showed the usual benefit of isolative prequestions on prequestioned but not on non-prequestioned material, but no benefit of integrative prequestions. However, in Experiment 2 when participants were given instructions to seek the answers while reading, integrative prequestions benefited learning of both prequestioned and non-prequestioned material. Individual differences in structure building positively predicted performance, but did not interact with the effects of prequestions.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Leitura , Ensino/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Rememoração Mental
2.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 10: 341, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30498440

RESUMO

Over the last few decades, considerable evidence shows that greater levels of aerobic exercise and cardiovascular fitness benefit cognitive performance. However, the degree to which free-living activity in community settings is related to cognitive performance remains unclear, particularly in older adults vulnerable to disability. Also, it is unknown whether the manner in which daily physical activity (PA) and sedentary time are accumulated throughout the day is associated with cognition. Cross-sectional associations between accelerometer-characterized PA and sedentary patterns and cognitive performance were examined in 1,274 mobility-limited older adults. Percent time spent in various bout lengths of PA (≥1, ≥2, and ≥5 min) and sedentary (≥1, ≥30, and ≥60 min) was defined as the number of minutes registered divided by total wear time × 100. Percent time was then tertiled for each bout length. Multiple linear regression models were used to estimate the associations between accelerometer bout variables and separate cognitive domains that included processing speed (Digit Symbol Coding; DSC), immediate and delayed recall (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; HVLT), information processing and selective attention (Flanker), working memory (n-back), reaction time (switch and non-switch reaction time), and a composite score that averaged results from all cognitive tests. After adjusting for demographics, behavioral factors, and morbid conditions, more time spent in PA was associated with higher DSC for all bout lengths (p < 0.03 for all). Higher PA was associated with higher HVLT and global cognition scores but only for longer bout lengths (p < 0.05 for all). The association was largely driven by participants who spent the lowest amount of time performing activity while awake (p < 0.04). An inverse linear relationship was observed between total sedentary time and DSC (p = 0.02), but not for other measures of cognition. These results suggest that, while higher PA was associated with higher cognitive performance, PA's association with memory was sensitive to bout duration. The time, but not the manner, spent in sedentary behaviors showed a minor association with executive function. Further research is warranted to characterize longitudinal changes in daily activity and sedentary patterns as potential biophysical markers of cognitive status in older adults.

3.
Psychol Aging ; 33(1): 93-108, 2018 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29494181

RESUMO

The objectives of this study were to replicate age-related decrements in recollection and source memory, and to determine if repetition lag training improves recollection and whether these effects maintain and transfer to other tasks. Sixteen young adults and 46 healthy older adults participated, the latter of whom comprised hi-old (n = 16) and lo-old (n = 30) based on neuropsychological memory tests. All participants completed memory tests and questionnaires at baseline, and then half of the lo-old underwent nine days of repetition lag training while the other half engaged in a 9-day active control program. The memory tests and questionnaires were repeated immediately after the training or control program, and again three months later. The baseline data replicated well-established age-related decrements in recollection. Repetition lag training improved objective measures of recollection, eliminated the age-related recollection decrement, and these improvements maintained over three months. However, training did not transfer to any other objective test of memory thought to rely on recollection, or to any subjective memory measure. The results demonstrate for the first time that repetition lag training improves objective measures of recollection, eliminates recollection differences between younger and older adults, and that these gains maintain over a 3-month period posttraining. The lack of transfer to other tasks, however, indicates that training one type of recollection (for the studied modality in this case) does not affect other types of recollection (e.g., of an item's recency). We suggest that recollection can be fractionated into many distinct types. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Transtornos da Memória/terapia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
4.
Brain Inj ; 31(1): 57-67, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27880059

RESUMO

PRIMARY OBJECTIVE: Repetition-lag memory training was developed to increase individuals' use of recollection as opposed to familiarity in recognition memory. The goals of this study were to examine the feasibility of repetition-lag training in patients with chronic stroke and to explore whether the training might show suggestions of transfer to non-trained tasks. RESEARCH DESIGN: Quasi-experimental. METHODS AND PROCEDURES: Patients (n = 17) took part in six repetition-lag training sessions and their gains on the training and non-trained tasks were compared to those of age-matched healthy controls (n = 30). MAIN OUTCOMES AND RESULTS: All but two patients completed the training, indicating that the method is feasible with a wide range of patients with stroke. The amount patients gained on the training task was similar to that of healthy controls (that is, the Group × Time interactions were by-and-large not significant), suggesting that patients with stroke might benefit to the same degree as healthy adults from this training. Both groups showed some indication of transfer to the non-trained backward digit span task and visuospatial memory. CONCLUSIONS: These findings show that repetition-lag memory training is a possible approach with patients with stroke to enhance recollection. Further research on the method's efficacy and effectiveness is warranted.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Transtornos da Memória/reabilitação , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Reabilitação do Acidente Vascular Cerebral/métodos , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/psicologia , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações , Resultado do Tratamento
6.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 898, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25477801

RESUMO

Normal aging holds negative consequences for memory, in particular for the ability to recollect the precise details of an experience. With this in mind, Jennings and Jacoby (2003) developed a recollection training method using a single-probe recognition memory paradigm in which new items (i.e., foils) were repeated during the test phase at increasingly long intervals. In previous reports, this method has appeared to improve older adults' performance on several non-trained cognitive tasks. We aimed to further examine potential transfer effects of this training paradigm and to determine which cognitive functions might predict training gains. Fifty-one older adults were assigned to either recollection training (n = 30) or an active control condition (n = 21) for six sessions over 2 weeks. Afterward, the recollection training group showed a greatly enhanced ability to reject the repeated foils. Surprisingly, however, the training and the control groups improved to the same degree in recognition accuracy (d') on their respective training tasks. Further, despite the recollection group's significant improvement in rejecting the repeated foils, we observed little evidence of transfer to non-trained tasks (including a temporal source memory test). Younger age and higher baseline scores on a measure of global cognitive function (as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment tool) and working memory (as measured by Digit Span Backward) predicted gains made by the recollection training group members.

7.
Clin Interv Aging ; 9: 1425-36, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25210447

RESUMO

Observational studies have shown beneficial relationships between exercise and cognitive function. Some clinical trials have also demonstrated improvements in cognitive function in response to moderate-high intensity aerobic exercise; however, these have been limited by relatively small sample sizes and short durations. The Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders (LIFE) Study is the largest and longest randomized controlled clinical trial of physical activity with cognitive outcomes, in older sedentary adults at increased risk for incident mobility disability. One LIFE Study objective is to evaluate the effects of a structured physical activity program on changes in cognitive function and incident all-cause mild cognitive impairment or dementia. Here, we present the design and baseline cognitive data. At baseline, participants completed the modified Mini Mental Status Examination, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Digit Symbol Coding, Modified Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure, and a computerized battery, selected to be sensitive to changes in speed of processing and executive functioning. During follow up, participants completed the same battery, along with the Category Fluency for Animals, Boston Naming, and Trail Making tests. The description of the mild cognitive impairment/dementia adjudication process is presented here. Participants with worse baseline Short Physical Performance Battery scores (prespecified at ≤ 7) had significantly lower median cognitive test scores compared with those having scores of 8 or 9 with modified Mini Mental Status Examination score of 91 versus (vs) 93, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test delayed recall score of 7.4 vs 7.9, and Digit Symbol Coding score of 45 vs 48, respectively (all P<0.001). The LIFE Study will contribute important information on the effects of a structured physical activity program on cognitive outcomes in sedentary older adults at particular risk for mobility impairment. In addition to its importance in the area of prevention of cognitive decline, the LIFE Study will also likely serve as a model for exercise and other behavioral intervention trials in older adults.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva/reabilitação , Terapia por Exercício/métodos , Limitação da Mobilidade , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Demência/prevenção & controle , Avaliação da Deficiência , Feminino , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Projetos de Pesquisa , Fatores de Risco , Comportamento Sedentário , Estados Unidos
8.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 28(12): 1239-50, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23589390

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Computer-administered assessment of cognitive function is being increasingly incorporated in clinical trials; however, its performance in these settings has not been systematically evaluated. DESIGN: The Seniors Health and Activity Research Program pilot trial (N = 73) developed a computer-based tool for assessing memory performance and executive functioning. The Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders investigators incorporated this battery in a full-scale multicenter clinical trial (N = 1635). We describe relationships that test scores have with those from interviewer-administered cognitive function tests and risk factors for cognitive deficits and describe performance measures (completeness, intraclass correlations [ICC]). RESULTS: Computer-based assessments of cognitive function had consistent relationships across the pilot and full-scale trial cohorts with interviewer-administered assessments of cognitive function, age, and a measure of physical function. In the Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders cohort, their external validity was further demonstrated by associations with other risk factors for cognitive dysfunction: education, hypertension, diabetes, and physical function. Acceptable levels of data completeness (>83%) were achieved on all computer-based measures; however, rates of missing data were higher among older participants (odds ratio = 1.06 for each additional year; p < 0.001) and those who reported no current computer use (odds ratio = 2.71; p < 0.001). ICCs among clinics were at least as low (ICC < 0.013) as for interviewer measures (ICC < 0.023), reflecting good standardization. All cognitive measures loaded onto the first principal component (global cognitive function), which accounted for 40% of the overall variance. CONCLUSION: Our results support the use of computer-based tools for assessing cognitive function in multicenter clinical trials of older individuals.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Diagnóstico por Computador , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/prevenção & controle , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Estilo de Vida , Masculino , Projetos Piloto , Fatores de Risco
9.
Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res ; 2013: 495793, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24454359

RESUMO

Intraindividual variability among cognitive domains may predict dementia independently of interindividual differences in cognition. A multidomain cognitive battery was administered to 2305 older adult women (mean age 74 years) enrolled in an ancillary study of the Women's Health Initiative. Women were evaluated annually for probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) for an average of 5.3 years using a standardized protocol. Proportional hazards regression showed that lower baseline domain-specific cognitive scores significantly predicted MCI (N = 74), probable dementia (N = 45), and MCI or probable dementia combined (N = 101) and that verbal and figural memory predicted each outcome independently of all other cognitive domains. The baseline intraindividual standard deviation across test scores (IAV Cognitive Domains) significantly predicted probable dementia and this effect was attenuated by interindividual differences in verbal episodic memory. Slope increases in IAV Cognitive Domains across measurement occasions (IAV Time) explained additional risk for MCI and MCI or probable dementia, beyond that accounted for by interindividual differences in multiple cognitive measures, but risk for probable dementia was attenuated by mean decreases in verbal episodic memory slope. These findings demonstrate that within-person variability across cognitive domains both at baseline and longitudinally independently accounts for risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in support of the predictive utility of within-person variability.

10.
Brain Cogn ; 78(2): 169-77, 2012 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22122949

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: This study was designed to extend the use of a memory training technique, known as the repetition-lag procedure, to Alzheimer patients. The specificity of this procedure is to target the process of recollection for improvement. METHOD: A group of 12 patients were trained individually for 6h. The training procedure consisted of a series of yes/no recognition tasks in which some words were repeated throughout the test list across gradually increasing delays. Their performance was evaluated on pre-and-post tests and compared with a recognition practice group and a no contact control group. RESULTS: Initially, recollection training patients only performed accurately when the delay between repetitions consisted of one intervening word, but by the end of training their performance increased up to four-word intervals. Interestingly, these benefits generalized to other measures of memory, such as working memory, visual memory and source recognition. CONCLUSIONS: Effectiveness of the repetition-lag procedure in Alzheimer's disease is discussed.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/terapia , Rememoração Mental , Transferência de Experiência , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Ensino/métodos , Fatores de Tempo , Resultado do Tratamento
11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21812705

RESUMO

The repetition-lag training procedure developed by Jennings and Jacoby (2003 , Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14, 417) has been shown to improve older adults' performance in the recognition memory task used for training, and to improve performance in a variety of other memory and executive function tasks ( Jennings, Webster, Kleykamp, & Dagenbach, 2005 , Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 12, 278). The present study examined the effects of concurrent interference tasks during the study or test phases of training to localize the source of gains. Overall, the results suggest that training is resilient and resistant to interference, but also that the processes used during the test phases of training are more important to the gains seen in the primary task and in the transfer tasks than those used in the study phases.


Assuntos
Técnicas Psicológicas , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Transferência de Experiência , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Atenção , Função Executiva , Humanos , Memória , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação
12.
BMC Geriatr ; 11: 27, 2011 May 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21615936

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The efficacy of non-pharmacological intervention approaches such as physical activity, strength, and cognitive training for improving brain health has not been established. Before definitive trials are mounted, important design questions on participation/adherence, training and interventions effects must be answered to more fully inform a full-scale trial. METHODS: SHARP-P was a single-blinded randomized controlled pilot trial of a 4-month physical activity training intervention (PA) and/or cognitive training intervention (CT) in a 2 × 2 factorial design with a health education control condition in 73 community-dwelling persons, aged 70-85 years, who were at risk for cognitive decline but did not have mild cognitive impairment. RESULTS: Intervention attendance rates were higher in the CT and PACT groups: CT: 96%, PA: 76%, PACT: 90% (p=0.004), the interventions produced marked changes in cognitive and physical performance measures (p≤0.05), and retention rates exceeded 90%. There were no statistically significant differences in 4-month changes in composite scores of cognitive, executive, and episodic memory function among arms. Four-month improvements in the composite measure increased with age among participants assigned to physical activity training but decreased with age for other participants (intervention*age interaction p=0.01). Depending on the choice of outcome, two-armed full-scale trials may require fewer than 1,000 participants (continuous outcome) or 2,000 participants (categorical outcome). CONCLUSIONS: Good levels of participation, adherence, and retention appear to be achievable for participants through age 85 years. Care should be taken to ensure that an attention control condition does not attenuate intervention effects. Depending on the choice of outcome measures, the necessary sample sizes to conduct four-year trials appear to be feasible. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00688155.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/terapia , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental/métodos , Aprendizagem , Atividade Motora , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cognitivos/prevenção & controle , Estudos de Coortes , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Projetos Piloto , Método Simples-Cego , Resultado do Tratamento
13.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol ; 19(1): 75-84, 2011 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21341925

RESUMO

Smokers experience cognitive decrements during tobacco abstinence and boosts in performance on resumption of smoking. Few studies have examined whether smoking cessation treatments such as transdermal nicotine (TN) ameliorate these decrements or attenuate the cognitive effects of smoking. Identifying the effects of nicotine on these tobacco-related changes in performance could guide the development of more efficacious treatments. The purpose of this double-blind, randomized, laboratory study was to use process-specific cognitive tasks to examine the effects of TN and tobacco smoking on attention and working memory in overnight-abstinent smokers (N = 124; 54 women). Each participant completed 4 sessions lasting 6.5 hr corresponding to 0-, 7-, 14-, or 21-mg TN doses, and smoked a single cigarette 4 hr after TN administration. Outcome measures were administered before and after smoking and included tasks measuring attention (alerting, orienting, and executive function), working memory (verbal and spatial), and psychomotor function. Analysis of variance (p < .05) revealed that TN improved verbal and spatial working memory performance, as well as psychomotor function. Smoking, independent of TN dose, improved alerting, verbal working memory, and psychomotor function. Lastly, TN partially attenuated the effects of smoking on some working memory outcomes. These findings lend evidence to the idea that TN ameliorates some abstinence-related cognitive decrements and suggest that TN does not completely attenuate the cognitive effects of a concurrently smoked cigarette. Consequently, TN's efficacy as a smoking cessation treatment might be improved if these limitations are better addressed by either modifying or supplementing existing treatments.


Assuntos
Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Memória de Curto Prazo/efeitos dos fármacos , Nicotina/administração & dosagem , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Fumar/psicologia , Tabagismo/psicologia , Administração Cutânea , Adulto , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fumar/fisiopatologia , Tabagismo/tratamento farmacológico , Tabagismo/fisiopatologia , Adulto Jovem
14.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 26(2): 135-43, 2011 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21229597

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To examine the performance of the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS) for identifying participants appropriate for trials of physical activity and cognitive training interventions. METHODS: Volunteers (N=343), ages 70-85 years, who were being recruited for a pilot clinical trial on approaches to prevent cognitive decline, were administered TICS and required to score ≥ 31 prior to an invitation to attend clinic-based assessments. The frequencies of contraindications for physical activity and cognitive training interventions were tallied for individuals grouped by TICS scores. Relationships between TICS scores and other measures of cognitive function were described by scatterplots and correlation coefficients. RESULTS: Eligibility criteria to identify candidates who were appropriate candidates for the trial interventions excluded 51.7% of the volunteers with TICS<31. TICS scores above this range were not strongly related to cognition or attendance at screening visits, however overall enrollment yields were approximately half for participants with TICS=31 versus TICS=41, and increased in a graded fashion throughout the range of scores. CONCLUSIONS: Use of TICS to define eligibility criteria in trials of physical activity and cognitive training interventions may not be worthwhile in that many individuals with low scores would already be eliminated by intervention-specific criteria and the relationship of TICS with clinic-based tests of cognitive function among appropriate candidates for these interventions may be weak. TICS may be most useful in these trials to identify candidates for oversampling in order to obtain a balanced cohort of participants at risk for cognitive decline.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/prevenção & controle , Cognição/fisiologia , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental , Exercício Físico/psicologia , Programas de Rastreamento/métodos , Esforço Físico , Consulta Remota/métodos , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Avaliação Geriátrica/métodos , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Projetos Piloto , Telefone
15.
Nitric Oxide ; 24(1): 34-42, 2011 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20951824

RESUMO

AIMS: Poor blood flow and hypoxia/ischemia contribute to many disease states and may also be a factor in the decline of physical and cognitive function in aging. Nitrite has been discovered to be a vasodilator that is preferentially harnessed in hypoxia. Thus, both infused and inhaled nitrite are being studied as therapeutic agents for a variety of diseases. In addition, nitrite derived from nitrate in the diet has been shown to decrease blood pressure and improve exercise performance. Thus, dietary nitrate may also be important when increased blood flow in hypoxic or ischemic areas is indicated. These conditions could include age-associated dementia and cognitive decline. The goal of this study was to determine if dietary nitrate would increase cerebral blood flow in older adults. METHODS AND RESULTS: In this investigation we administered a high vs. low nitrate diet to older adults (74.7±6.9 years) and measured cerebral perfusion using arterial spin labeling magnetic resonance imaging. We found that the high nitrate diet did not alter global cerebral perfusion, but did lead to increased regional cerebral perfusion in frontal lobe white matter, especially between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. CONCLUSION: These results suggest that dietary nitrate may be useful in improving regional brain perfusion in older adults in critical brain areas known to be involved in executive functioning.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/irrigação sanguínea , Nitratos/administração & dosagem , Nitritos/sangue , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Suplementos Nutricionais , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Nitratos/sangue , Fluxo Sanguíneo Regional/efeitos dos fármacos
16.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 2: 23, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20589103

RESUMO

Literature has shown that exercise is beneficial for cognitive function in older adults and that aerobic fitness is associated with increased hippocampal tissue and blood volumes. The current study used novel network science methods to shed light on the neurophysiological implications of exercise-induced changes in the hippocampus of older adults. Participants represented a volunteer subgroup of older adults that were part of either the exercise training (ET) or healthy aging educational control (HAC) treatment arms from the Seniors Health and Activity Research Program Pilot (SHARP-P) trial. Following the 4-month interventions, MRI measures of resting brain blood flow and connectivity were performed. The ET group's hippocampal cerebral blood flow (CBF) exhibited statistically significant increases compared to the HAC group. Novel whole-brain network connectivity analyses showed greater connectivity in the hippocampi of the ET participants compared to HAC. Furthermore, the hippocampus was consistently shown to be within the same network neighborhood (module) as the anterior cingulate cortex only within the ET group. Thus, within the ET group, the hippocampus and anterior cingulate were highly interconnected and localized to the same network neighborhood. This project shows the power of network science to investigate potential mechanisms for exercise-induced benefits to the brain in older adults. We show a link between neurological network features and CBF, and it is possible that this alteration of functional brain networks may lead to the known improvement in cognitive function among older adults following exercise.

17.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol ; 16(2): 99-112, 2008 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18489014

RESUMO

Transdermal nicotine (TN) is an efficacious smoking cessation pharmacotherapy thought to work, in part, by attenuating the effects of tobacco/nicotine abstinence and the effects of concurrently smoked cigarettes. Clinical trials suggest that TN may be less efficacious for women. This study explored the possibility of TN-related gender differences in > or = 8 hour abstinent smokers (54 women, 70 men) who completed four within-subject, double-blind, placebo-controlled sessions corresponding to 0, 7, 14, and 21 mg TN. In each approximately 6.5-hr long session participants smoked an own-brand cigarette 4 hours after TN administration and physiological and subjective outcomes were examined throughout each session. Results revealed that TN suppressed some signs and symptoms of tobacco abstinence and attenuated some effects of smoking, and these effects were not dependent on gender. Women were more sensitive to the direct effects of nicotine (e.g., ratings of Nauseous) and, independent of TN dose, self-administered less nicotine when smoking and rated smoking as less rewarding. Thus, although this study does not shed light on clinical observations that TN is less effective for women, results suggest that TN might need to be combined with other interventions to supplement its effects on tobacco/nicotine abstinence and concurrent smoking.


Assuntos
Nicotina/administração & dosagem , Agonistas Nicotínicos/administração & dosagem , Caracteres Sexuais , Fumar/psicologia , Tabagismo/tratamento farmacológico , Administração Cutânea , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual , Nicotina/efeitos adversos , Nicotina/sangue , Agonistas Nicotínicos/efeitos adversos , Agonistas Nicotínicos/sangue , Abandono do Uso de Tabaco , Tabagismo/fisiopatologia , Tabagismo/psicologia
18.
Neuropsychology ; 22(2): 177-87, 2008 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18331160

RESUMO

Little is known about the cognitive mechanisms of the memory impairment associated with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). We explored recollection and familiarity in 27 healthy young adults, 45 healthy older adults, and 17 individuals with aMCI. Relative to the younger adults, recollection was reduced in the older adults, especially among those with aMCI. Familiarity did not differ among groups. In the healthy younger and older adults, better performance on a set of clinical memory measures that are sensitive to medial temporal lobe functioning was associated with greater recollection. In addition, among the healthy older adults better executive functioning was also associated with greater recollection. These results are consistent with the notion that recollection is a product of strategic processes mediated by the prefrontal cortex that suppport the retrieval of context-dependent memories from the hippocampus. Hippocampal atrophy associated with aMCI may disrupt this brain network, and thereby interfere with recollection.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Amnésia/psicologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Memória/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Ansiedade/psicologia , Depressão/psicologia , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Psicolinguística , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Leitura , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Vocabulário
19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17612813

RESUMO

The effects of aging on alerting, orienting, and executive function were examined with the use of the Attention Network Task, which combines the Posner spatial cuing task and the Eriksen flanker task into a single procedure. We found that older adults showed significantly less alerting than young adults in response to a warning cue, although there were no age differences in orienting or executive function once processing speed was taken into account. We suggest that age differences in alerting may depend in part upon the presentation duration or persistence of the warning cue.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Atenção , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
20.
Am J Health Behav ; 31(5): 473-83, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17555378

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine if nicotine's effects are influenced by caffeine in nonsmoking, moderate-caffeine consuming individuals (N=20). METHODS: The first 3 sessions included one of 3 randomly ordered, double-blind caffeine doses (0, 75, or 150 mg, oral [po]) and 2 single-blind nicotine gum doses (2 and 4 mg) in ascending order. The fourth session (single blind) repeated the 0 mg caffeine condition. RESULTS: Nicotine increased heart rate and subjective ratings indicative of aversive effects, and decreased reaction times. These effects were independent of caffeine dose and reliable across sessions. CONCLUSIONS: In nonsmokers, nicotine effects are not influenced by moderate caffeine doses.


Assuntos
Afeto/efeitos dos fármacos , Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Pressão Sanguínea/efeitos dos fármacos , Cafeína/farmacologia , Frequência Cardíaca/efeitos dos fármacos , Nicotina/farmacologia , Tempo de Reação/efeitos dos fármacos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Atitude , Goma de Mascar , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Método Duplo-Cego , Interações Medicamentosas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Método Simples-Cego
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...