Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 2 de 2
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38149680

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Pictures drawn by people with aphasia (PWA) are often more challenging to understand than those drawn by healthy people. There are two types of objects: those that tend to be drawn symbolically (symbolically drawn objects-SOs) and those that are likely to be drawn realistically (realistically drawn objects-ROs). AIMS: To compare the identification rate and number of misunderstanding types between SOs and ROs drawn by PWA and healthy controls (HCs). To reveal trends in the misunderstandings of drawings by PWA, and to identify the language or cognitive abilities related to the identification rate of pictures drawn by PWA. METHODS & PROCEDURES: We designed a drawing task involving SOs and ROs. A total of 18 PWA and 30 HCs completed the task, and respondents identified the drawings. The identification rate and number of misunderstandings were analysed with two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) including group (PWA and HCs) and object type (SOs and ROs). The misunderstandings were divided into four categories varying in semantic and morphological similarity; these ratios were examined with a chi-square test. The relationships of language and cognitive abilities with the identification rate were investigated with multiple regression analyses. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: There was a significant effect of the interaction between group and object type on the identification rate (F(1.1387) = 3.90, Mean Squared Error (MSE) = 4139.67, p = 0.04): the identification rates for ROs were lower in the PWA than in the HCs. For the number of misunderstanding types, an interaction was observed between group and object type (F(1.56) = 8.26, MSE = 26.93, p < 0.01): the number of misunderstanding types for ROs in the PWA was greater than that in the HCs. The misunderstanding patterns differed between ROs and SOs (χ2 (3) = 694.30, p < 0.001, V = 0.37). ROs were semantically related, whereas SOs were morphologically related. The identification rates of ROs and SOs were correlated only with Kanji writing scores (ROs: ß = 3.66, p = 0.01; SOs: ß = 6.57, p < 0.01). CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: In drawings by the PWA, SOs had a higher identification rate, while ROs had a lower identification rate and a greater variety of misunderstandings. SOs may increase drawing motivation. Interventions to improve the identifiability of SOs and ROs should reflect each character. Identification rates were correlated only with Kanji writing scores. The PWA, whose native language was Japanese and had preserved Kanji writing abilities, and their communication abilities may be increased through drawing. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on the subject PWA often have impaired drawing abilities and draw pictures that third parties misinterpret. Some objects tend to be drawn symbolically, and some are drawn realistically. However, it is not clear whether there is a difference between these types of drawings depicted by PWA in identifiability and the tendency to be misunderstood by ordinary people. In addition, the relationships between language or cognitive abilities and the identification rate of drawn pictures are not clear. What this paper adds to the existing knowledge The identification rate differed between SOs and ROs. In drawings by PWA, SOs had a higher identification rate, while ROs had a lower identification rate and the greatest variety of misunderstandings. Approximately half of the misunderstandings were related to the target object. SOs tended to be confused with morphologically related objects, while ROs tended to be confused with semantically related objects. Identification rates were correlated only with Kanji writing scores. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? To motivate PWA's drawing, it is suitable to begin with SOs. Examining drawing ability from the perspective of SOs and ROs increases the chance of identifying drawing ability. PWA whose native language is Japanese and have preserved Kanji writing abilities may be able to increase their communication abilities through drawing.

2.
Nihon Ronen Igakkai Zasshi ; 59(1): 102-109, 2022.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35264524

RESUMO

In recent years, the assessment of and support for the safety of driving for people with higher brain dysfunction to allow them to resume car driving have become issues to be addressed in Japan. It is difficult to determine whether or not people with higher brain dysfunction may safely resume car driving; in addition, methods of supporting this resumption have not been established. To support people with higher brain dysfunction and allow them to live at home in areas where public means of transportation may be insufficient, initiatives promoting the resumption of car driving are necessary in healthcare sectors, including day rehabilitation facilities. We provided support to a patient with an attention disorder due to left thalamic infarction, with the aim of achieving sufficient independence to drive a car, in a day rehabilitation facility. We herein report this case from the perspective of a speech-language-hearing therapist. The patient was a right-handed man in his 60s who had higher brain dysfunction with attention disorder as the main symptom. No marked motor paralysis of the extremities was observed. Use of a day rehabilitation service was started approximately two months after the onset of symptoms. Rehabilitation and support aimed at the resumption of car driving were provided approximately one month after the start of the day rehabilitation service use. To determine whether or not the patient was fit to drive a car, higher brain function tests for the intellectual function, attention function, and frontal function, as well as a theoretical evaluation based on the Stroke Drivers' Screening Assessment Japanese Version (J-SDSA) and monitoring of daily behaviors were performed. In addition, after the patient was given permission from an attending physician to drive a car on the condition that the patient did not drive fast and the patient's wife always accompanied him while driving, a safety assessment was also performed. As a result, approximately 10 months later, the J-SDSA theoretical evaluation score showed a passing grade, in contrast to the failing grade he had previously earned. Furthermore, errors in performing household activities due to a decreased attention function became unremarkable with respect to daily behaviors; therefore, we determined, together with the attending physician, that the patient now had sufficient independence to drive a car. In our day rehabilitation facility, the number of requests for advice on car driving from people with higher brain dysfunction living in the community had been increasing. Multisectoral assessments, training, and instruction should be continued in collaboration with attending physicians, other facilities located within the community, and driving schools in order to support people with higher brain dysfunction and help them once again be able to drive a car.


Assuntos
Condução de Veículo , Transtornos Cognitivos , Infarto Cerebral , Humanos , Japão , Masculino , Meios de Transporte
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...