Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 3 de 3
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Ment Health ; 28(3): 255-259, 2019 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27885883

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Stigma associated with mental illness (MI) results in underutilization of mental health care. We must understand factors contributing to stigma to shape anti-stigma campaigns. AIMS: To investigate the factors influencing stigma in university students. METHOD: Undergraduate psychology students completed measures on causal attribution, stigma, social distance, implicit person theory (IPT), and familiarity. RESULTS: The hypothesis was partially supported; people who felt personality traits were unchangeable (i.e. entity IPT) were more likely to stigmatize individuals with mental disorders and desired more social distance from them. Familiarity with people with a MI individually predicted less desire for social distance, yet the redundancy of the predictors made the effect of familiarity on stigma fall just short of statistical significance. Judgments of biogenetic causal attribution were related to higher stigma levels, but not so when familiarity and IPT were taken into account. CONCLUSIONS: Educational campaigns may be effective by focusing on aspects of MI highlighting similarity with non-diagnosed people, and that people with MI can recover.


Assuntos
Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Transtornos Mentais/psicologia , Estigma Social , Estudantes/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Mentais/prevenção & controle , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Personalidade , Distância Psicológica , Universidades , Adulto Jovem
2.
Aggress Behav ; 37(6): 503-20, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21932334

RESUMO

This study tested the I(3) model [Finkel, 2007; 2008] of intimate partner violence as applied to obsessive relational intrusion (ORI) to assess the relation among self-regulation, rejection, rejection sensitivity (RS), and stalking-related aggression. In Study 1, participants (N=221) read one of three vignettes: no relationship termination, an "internal" rejection (involves an internal attribution to the rejected as cause of relationship ending), or an "external" rejection (external attributions for relationship demise). Next, participants experienced one of two conditions manipulating self-regulation (no depletion vs. depletion). Finally, participants rated their likelihood of engaging in ORI (e.g. unwanted pursuit and/or aggression). Consistent with predictions, participants receiving an internal rejection reported higher aggression than participants experiencing an external rejection, especially when depleted of self-regulation. Study 2 extended the design of Study 1 by adding in a screening survey of RS. Internal rejections still yielded more aggression than other conditions, but this was especially so when rejection-sensitive persons were depleted of self-regulation. In addition to providing support for the I(3) model of aggression, this research shows that not all types of rejection are created equal.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Rejeição em Psicologia , Autoimagem , Controles Informais da Sociedade , Perseguição/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
3.
Aggress Behav ; 33(4): 291-303, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17593561

RESUMO

This study examines the relationship between perpetrator characteristics, situational characteristics, and type of sexual coercion tactics used to obtain sexual contact (including sexual intercourse) with an unwilling partner. Men who used manipulation or force were compared to each other and to men who engaged in only consensual sex. Participants were college men drawn from the first wave of a 5-year longitudinal study. Stepwise discriminant function analyses, univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA), and chi(2) analyses tested group differences. As predicted, men who used force reported more childhood sexual abuse, witnessed more domestic violence, were more accepting of male violence, and were less likely to endorse love as a motive for sex than men in both the manipulation and consent groups. Men in the force group were also more likely to have had a casual relationship with the woman, and to be drinking and also intoxicated during the coercive incident than men in the manipulation group. Hypothesized differences between men who used force and manipulation regarding parental physical punishment, traditional gender role attitudes, delinquency, hedonistic and dominance motives for sex, prior sexual contact, and the length of the relationship were not supported. The results suggest that types of tactic used in sexual assaults can be distinguished on the basis of person and situational variables and that knowledge of these differences can facilitate future research, as well as rape deterrent and intervention programs.


Assuntos
Coerção , Corte , Estupro , Comportamento Sexual , Adolescente , Adulto , Intoxicação Alcoólica/epidemiologia , Intoxicação Alcoólica/psicologia , Análise de Variância , Atitude , Criança , Maus-Tratos Infantis/psicologia , Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Análise Discriminante , Humanos , Delinquência Juvenil/psicologia , Delinquência Juvenil/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino , Motivação , Estupro/prevenção & controle , Estupro/psicologia , Estupro/estatística & dados numéricos , Fatores de Risco , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Comportamento Sexual/estatística & dados numéricos , Sudeste dos Estados Unidos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...