Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
PLoS One ; 19(3): e0300200, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38452146

RESUMO

Sharing of resources is a common feature of human societies. Yet, there is substantial societal variation in children's generosity, and this variation emerges during middle childhood. Societal differences in self-construal orientation may be one factor influencing the ontogeny of generosity. Here, we examine anonymous Dictator Game sharing in 7-and-8-year-olds from two distinct societies: India and the UK (N = 180). We used self-construal manipulations to investigate whether priming self- or other-focused conversations would differentially influence children's generosity. There were no differences in generosity between populations. While a significant reduction in generosity was found following self-priming in both societies, other-priming was ineffectual. The findings are discussed in relation to experimental features and the role of anonymity and reputational concerns.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Humanos , Criança , Índia
2.
Aging Med (Milton) ; 6(2): 116-123, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37287675

RESUMO

Introduction: Polypharmacy is a growing phenomenon associated with adverse effects in older adults. We assessed the potential confounding effects of cumulative anticholinergic burden (ACB) in patients who were hospitalized with falls. Methods: A noninterventional, prospective cohort study of unselected, acute admissions aged ≥ 65 years. Data were derived from electronic patient health records. Results were analyzed to determine the frequency of polypharmacy and degree of ACB and their relationship to falls risk. Primary outcomes were polypharmacy, defined as prescription of 5 or more regular oral medications, and ACB score. Key Results: Four hundred eleven (411) consecutive subjects were included, mean age 83.8 ± 8.0 years: 40.6% men. There were 38.4% patients who were admitted with falls. Incidence of polypharmacy was 80.8%, (88.0% and 76.3% among those admitted with and without fall, respectively). Incidence of ACB score of 0, 1, 2, ≥ 3 was 38.7%, 20.9%, 14.6%, and 25.8%, respectively. On multivariate analysis, age [odds ratio (OR) = 1.030, 95% CI:1.000 ~ 1.050, P = 0.049], ACB score (OR = 1.150, 95% CI:1.020 ~ 1.290, P = 0.025), polypharmacy (OR = 2.140, 95% CI:1.190 ~ 3.870, P = 0.012), but not Charlson Comorbidity Index (OR = 0.920, 95% CI:0.810 ~ 1.040, P = 0.172) were significantly associated with higher falls rate. Of patients admitted with falls, 29.8% had drug-related orthostatic hypotension, 24.7% had drug-related bradycardia, 37.3% were prescribed centrally acting drugs, and 12.0% were taking inappropriate hypoglycemic agents. Conclusion: Polypharmacy results in cumulative ACB and both are significantly associated with falls risk in older adults. The presence of polypharmacy and each unit rise in ACB score have a stronger effect of increasing falls risk compared to age and comorbidities.

3.
Dev Psychol ; 58(4): 662-670, 2022 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35343714

RESUMO

Creating objects can increase our evaluation of them, even when we compare them to physically identical copies (IKEA effect). Here we evaluate the influence of collaboration on the IKEA effect in two societies-the United Kingdom and India. One hundred twenty-eight 5-to-6-year-old children (48% female, 50% British middle class, 50% Indian middle class) assembled toys in pairs. Half of the children collaborated to assemble a single toy and half assembled their own toy. In both societies, children demonstrated an IKEA effect (η2p = .19), valuing their own creation over an identical copy. This was the case regardless of whether children collaborated or worked independently. In summary, it seems that the IKEA effect is a potent bias that is present in diverse societies and is insensitive to others' contributions in a collaborative environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Psicologia da Criança , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Índia , Masculino , Jogos e Brinquedos , Reino Unido
4.
Health Hum Rights ; 22(2): 155-166, 2020 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33390704

RESUMO

This study presents qualitative results from a mixed-method investigation conducted between May and August 2012 into the prevalence and consequences of four forms of intimate partner violence among women living with HIV who attended the Comprehensive Care Clinic at the Kenyatta National Hospital in Nairobi. As a part of the research, a quantitative survey found that among 600 sexually active women living with HIV aged 18-69, all reported experiencing emotional abuse; 20%, 17%, and 15% experienced controlling behavior, physical violence, and sexual violence, respectively. Qualitative research using focus group discussions with 19 women from the quantitative survey sought to contextualize these experiences and place them within a larger social structure where institutionalized gender inequality sets the tone for intimate partner violence against women in households. Participants reported that intimate partner violence led to their exposure to the virus and made them leery of disclosing their positive status or seeking support from a male partner for fear of a violent reaction. This fear and the socio-structural conditions in Kenya limit their ability to actively pursue comprehensive care, the stress of which can exacerbate symptoms and make managing the disease more difficult.


Assuntos
Infecções por HIV , Violência por Parceiro Íntimo , Delitos Sexuais , Feminino , Infecções por HIV/epidemiologia , Direitos Humanos , Humanos , Quênia/epidemiologia , Masculino
5.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 184: 139-157, 2019 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31034994

RESUMO

Most humans share to some degree. Yet, from middle childhood, sharing behavior varies substantially across societies. Here, for the first time, we explored the effect of self-construal manipulation on sharing decisions in 7- and 8-year-old children from two distinct societies: urban India and urban United Kingdom. Children participated in one of three conditions that focused attention on independence, interdependence, or a control. Sharing was then assessed across three resource allocation games. A focus on independence resulted in reduced generosity in both societies. However, an intriguing societal difference emerged following a focus on interdependence, where only Indian children from traditional extended families displayed greater generosity in one of the resource allocation games. Thus, a focus on independence can move children from diverse societies toward selfishness with relative ease, but a focus on interdependence is very limited in its effectiveness to promote generosity.


Assuntos
Tomada de Decisões , Alocação de Recursos , Comportamento Social , Criança , Comparação Transcultural , Feminino , Humanos , Índia , Masculino , Reino Unido
6.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 180: 123-130, 2019 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30655097

RESUMO

This study tested whether overimitation is subject to an audience effect, and whether it is modulated by object novelty. A sample of 86 4- to 11-year-old children watched a demonstrator open novel and familiar boxes using sequences of necessary and unnecessary actions. The experimenter then observed the children, turned away, or left the room while the children opened the box. Children copied unnecessary actions more when the experimenter watched or when she left, but they copied less when she turned away. This parallels infant studies suggesting that turning away is interpreted as a signal of disengagement. Children displayed increased overimitation and reduced efficiency discrimination when opening novel boxes compared with familiar boxes. These data provide important evidence that object novelty is a critical component of overimitation.


Assuntos
Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Aprendizagem , Masculino
7.
PLoS One ; 13(1): e0189752, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29320506

RESUMO

By 7-to 8-years of age, most children readily adhere to prosocial norms aimed at benefiting others through giving up time and effort (helping) or resources (sharing). Two studies explored whether sharing and helping by 7-to 8-year olds (N = 180) could be influenced by priming children's attention on themselves or their friends through a semi-structured interview. Results revealed that self-priming led to reductions in both sharing and helping compared to friendship-priming or a control condition. These findings are considered as indicative of the fragile state of prosocial behaviours at this age that can be easily shifted towards more selfish biases by simple priming.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Ajuda , Comportamento Social , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Cognition ; 170: 245-253, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29080469

RESUMO

We elevate our constructions to a special status in our minds. This 'IKEA' effect leads us to believe that our creations are more valuable than items that are identical, but constructed by another. This series of studies utilises a developmental perspective to explore why this bias exists. Study 1 elucidates the ontogeny of the IKEA effect, demonstrating an emerging bias at age 5, corresponding with key developmental milestones in self-concept formation. Study 2 assesses the role of effort, revealing that the IKEA effect is not moderated by the amount of effort invested in the task in 5-to-6-year olds. Finally, Study 3 examines whether feelings of ownership moderate the IKEA effect, finding that ownership alone cannot explain why children value their creations more. Altogether, results from this study series are incompatible with existing theories of the IKEA bias. Instead, we propose a new framework to examine biases in decision making. Perhaps the IKEA effect reflects a link between our creations and our self-concept, emerging at age 5, leading us to value them more positively than others' creations.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Propriedade , Desempenho Psicomotor , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Neuroimage ; 139: 368-375, 2016 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27369095

RESUMO

Imitation has been hailed as 'social glue', facilitating rapport with others. Previous studies suggest that social cues modulate imitation but the mechanism of such modulation remains underspecified. Here we examine the locus, specificity, and neural basis of the social control of imitation. Social cues (group membership and eye gaze) were manipulated during an imitation task in which imitative and spatial compatibility could be measured independently. Participants were faster to perform compatible compared to incompatible movements in both spatial and imitative domains. However, only spatial compatibility was modulated by social cues: an interaction between group membership and eye gaze revealed more spatial compatibility for ingroup members with direct gaze and outgroup members with averted gaze. The fMRI data were consistent with this finding. Regions associated with the control of imitative responding (temporoparietal junction, inferior frontal gyrus) were more active during imitatively incompatible compared to imitatively compatible trials. However, this activity was not modulated by social cues. On the contrary, an interaction between group, gaze and spatial compatibility was found in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in a pattern consistent with reaction times. This region may be exerting control over the motor system to modulate response inhibition.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Comportamento Imitativo , Comportamento Social , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
10.
Cognition ; 152: 70-77, 2016 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27035610

RESUMO

When an object comes into possession, the owner will typically think that it is worth more than it did before they owned the item in a bias known as the endowment effect. This bias is particularly robust in Western societies with independent self-construals, but has not been observed in children below 5-6years of age. In three studies, we investigated whether endowment effect can be induced in younger children by focusing their attention on themselves. 120 children aged 3-4years evaluated toys before and after a task where they made pictures of themselves, a friend or a neutral farm scene. Over the three studies, children consistently evaluated their own possessions, relative to other identical toys, more positively following the self-priming manipulation. Together these studies support the notion that possessions can form part of an "extended self" from early on in development and that the endowment effect may be due to an attentional self-bias framing.


Assuntos
Atenção , Comportamento de Escolha , Autoimagem , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
PLoS One ; 11(4): e0153385, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27073864

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Development during the first two years of life is critical and has a lasting impact on a child's health. Poor infant and child nutrition can lead to deficiencies in essential micronutrients, which may cause a weakened immune system and lasting effects on children's growth and development. Recent studies in rural Shaanxi Province found an anemia prevalence of 54.3% among rural children aged six to twelve months. While new large-scale, quantitative research has begun to catalogue the extent of child malnutrition and anemia, no effort has yet been made to look more closely at the potential reasons for rural children's nutritional deficiencies through qualitative analysis. This study aims to elucidate some of the fundamental causes of poor complementary feeding practices that may lead to anemia among children in rural Shaanxi Province, China. METHODOLOGY: We interviewed sixty caregivers participating in a large survey on child health and nutrition. We conducted three waves of interviews with children's primary caregivers in seventeen rural villages within four nationally-designated poverty counties in the southern part of Shaanxi Province. RESULTS: The qualitative analysis reveals that poor complementary feeding practices are common across our sample. Information gathered from our interviews suggests that complementary feeding practices are impeded by two constraints: absence of understanding topics related to infant health and nutrition under caregivers, as well as inadequate sources of information on these topics. Poverty does not appear to constrain child feeding practices. CONCLUSION: Our results uncover lack of proper knowledge on infant and child nutrition among rural caregivers in China. This situation causes them to fail incorporating micronutrient rich foods in their children's diet. Age-appropriate complementary feeding can stimulate children's physical and cognitive development, but in its absence it leads to iron-deficiency anemia. We suggest that steps be taken to educate caregivers to improve complementary feeding of their infants and children.


Assuntos
Anemia Ferropriva/epidemiologia , Cuidadores , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Transtornos da Nutrição Infantil/epidemiologia , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Nutrição do Lactente/fisiologia , Anemia Ferropriva/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Nutrição Infantil/fisiopatologia , Pré-Escolar , China/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estado Nutricional , Prevalência , Saúde da População Rural , População Rural , Fatores Socioeconômicos
12.
Autism Res ; 9(1): 121-30, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26052836

RESUMO

Previous research has suggested that people with Autism Spectrum Conditions (ASC) may have difficulty with visual perspective taking (VPT) but it is not clear how this relates to different strategies that can be used in perspective taking tasks. The current study examined VPT in 30 children with autism and 30 verbal mental age matched typical children, in comparison to mental rotation (MR) abilities and body representation abilities. Using a similar paradigm to Hamilton, Brindley, and Frith [2009] all children completed three tasks: a VPT task in which children decided what a toy on a table would look like from a different points of view; a MR task in which the child decided what a toy would look like after it had been rotated; and a body posture matching task, in which children matched pictures of a body shown from different viewpoints. Results showed that children with ASC performed better than the typically developing children on the MR task, and at a similar level on the VPT task and body matching task. Importantly, in the typical children VPT performance was predicted by performance on the body matching task, whereas in the ASC children VPT performance was predicted by MR ability. These findings suggest that differences in VPT in ASC may be explained by the use of a spatial rotation strategy rather than the embodied egocentric transformation strategy used by typical children.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista/complicações , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/complicações , Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/psicologia , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
13.
Neuroimage ; 103: 81-90, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25241085

RESUMO

By observing other people, we can often infer goals and motivations behind their actions. This study examines the role of the action observation network (AON) and the mentalising network (MZN) in the perception of rational and irrational actions. Past studies in this area report mixed results, so the present paper uses new stimuli which precisely control motion path, the social form of the actor and the rationality of the action. A cluster in medial prefrontal cortex and a large cluster in the right inferior parietal lobule extending to the temporoparietal junction distinguished observation of irrational from rational actions. Activity within the temporoparietal region also correlated on a trial-by-trial basis with each participant's judgement of action rationality. These findings demonstrate that observation of another person performing an irrational action engages both action observation and mentalising networks. Our results advance current theories of action comprehension and the roles of action observation and mentalising networks in this process.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Compreensão/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
14.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 44(10): 2430-9, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24710812

RESUMO

It has been proposed that mentalising involves retrodicting as well as predicting behaviour, by inferring previous mental states of a target. This study investigated whether retrodiction is impaired in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Participants watched videos of real people reacting to the researcher behaving in one of four possible ways. Their task was to decide which of these four "scenarios" each person responded to. Participants' eye movements were recorded. Participants with ASD were poorer than comparison participants at identifying the scenario to which people in the videos were responding. There were no group differences in time spent looking at the eyes or mouth. The findings imply those with ASD are impaired in using mentalising skills for retrodiction.


Assuntos
Transtornos Globais do Desenvolvimento Infantil/psicologia , Teoria da Mente , Adolescente , Criança , Transtornos Globais do Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiopatologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Humanos , Masculino , Gravação de Videoteipe , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 44(9): 2277-89, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24658868

RESUMO

Previous research into autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has shown people with autism to be impaired at visual perspective taking. However it is still unclear to what extent the spatial mechanisms underlying this ability contribute to these difficulties. In the current experiment we examine spatial transformations in adults with ASD and typical adults. Participants performed egocentric transformations and mental rotation of bodies and cars. Results indicated that participants with ASD had general perceptual differences impacting on response times across tasks. However, they also showed more specific differences in the egocentric task suggesting particular difficulty with using the self as a reference frame. These findings suggest that impaired perspective taking could be grounded in difficulty with the spatial transformation used to imagine the self in someone else's place.


Assuntos
Transtornos Globais do Desenvolvimento Infantil/psicologia , Percepção Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Imaginação , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
16.
PLoS One ; 9(1): e86127, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24465913

RESUMO

Children copy the actions of others with high fidelity, even when they are not causally relevant. This copying of visibly unnecessary actions is termed overimitation. Many competing theories propose mechanisms for overimitation behaviour. The present study examines these theories by studying the social factors that lead children to overimitate actions. Ninety-four children aged 5- to 8-years each completed five trials of an overimitation task. Each trial provided the opportunity to overimitate an action on familiar objects with minimal causal reasoning demands. Social cues (live or video demonstration) and eye contact from the demonstrator were manipulated. After the imitation, children's ratings of action rationality were collected. Substantial overimitation was seen which increased with age. In older children, overimitation was higher when watching a live demonstrator and when eye contact was absent. Actions rated as irrational were more likely to be imitated than those rated as rational. Children overimitated actions on familiar objects even when they rated those actions as irrational, suggesting that failure of causal reasoning cannot be driving overimitation. Our data support social explanations of overimitation and show that the influence of social factors increases with age over the 5- to 8-year-old age range.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil , Comportamento Imitativo , Comportamento Social , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos
17.
Neuroimage ; 56(3): 1511-9, 2011 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21310248

RESUMO

The role of mirror neuron systems and mentalising systems in causing poor social and communication skills in individuals with autistic spectrum conditions is hotly debated. We studied 18 adults with autistic spectrum conditions in comparison to 19 age and IQ matched typical individuals. Behavioural assessments revealed difficulties in mental state attribution and action comprehension in the autism sample. We examined brain responses when observing rational and irrational hand actions, because these actions engage mirror and mentalising components of the social brain respectively. Both typical and autistic participants activated the left anterior intraparietal sulcus component of the mirror system when viewing hand actions compared to moving shapes. The typical but not autistic participants activated the posterior mid cingulate cortex/supplementary motor area and bilateral fusiform cortex when viewing hand actions. When viewing irrational hand actions, the medial prefrontal cortex of typical participants deactivated but this region did not distinguish the different stimuli in autistic participants. These results suggest that parietal mirror regions function normally in autism, while differences in action understanding could be due to abnormal function of cingulate, fusiform and medial prefrontal regions. Thus, brain regions associated with mirroring and mentalising functions are differentially affected in autistic spectrum conditions.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Processos Mentais , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Adulto , Síndrome de Asperger/psicologia , Comunicação , Compreensão/fisiologia , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Gestos , Objetivos , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...