Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Front Psychol ; 11: 103, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32082231

RESUMO

Previous studies have suggested that cognitive reappraisal, which is an effective emotion regulation strategy, enhances decentering. On the other hand, other studies have implied the reverse in regard to this relationship: that decentering supports cognitive reappraisal. However, these possibilities have not yet been examined empirically. In the present study, we investigated the causal relationship between decentering and cognitive reappraisal by conducting two wave cross-lagged analysis. One hundred and thirty-eight Japanese university students responded to a questionnaire comprising measures of decentering and cognitive reappraisal tendency; the questionnaire was administered on two occasions, with an interval of 1 month. Cross-lagged analysis indicated the positive effect of cognitive reappraisal on decentering; however, the reverse possibility, that decentering influences cognitive reappraisal, was not significant. These results suggested that habitual use of cognitive reappraisal fosters decentering.

2.
Neuroimage ; 201: 116015, 2019 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31306772

RESUMO

Our personal internal preferences while making decisions are usually consistent. Recent psychological studies, however, show observable variability of internal criteria occurs by random noise. The neural correlates of said random noise - an instance of 'psychological noise' - yet remain unclear. Combining simulation, behavioral, and neural approaches, our study investigated the psychological and neural correlates of such random noise in our internal criteria during decision making. We applied well-established decision-making tasks which relied on either internal criteria - occupation choice task as internally-guided decision making (IDM) - or external criteria - salary judgment task as externally-guided decision making (EDM). Subjects underwent EEG for resting state and task-evoked activity during IDM and EDM. We measured resting state long-range temporal correlation (LRTC) in the alpha frequency range as the index of neuronal noise. Based on our simulation, we identified a measure of psychological noise (as distinguished from true preference change) in IDM. The main finding shows that the indices for psychological noise are directly related to frontocentral LRTC in the alpha range. Higher degrees of frontocentral LRTC, which index lower neuronal noise, were related to lower degrees of psychological noise during IDM. This was not found during EDM. Resting state LRTC was also related to task-evoked activity, such as conflict-related negativity, during IDM only. Taken together, our data demonstrate, for the first time, the direct relationship between neuronal noise in the brain's intrinsic activity and psychological noise in the internal criteria of our decision making.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Adolescente , Atenção/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
3.
Front Psychol ; 10: 599, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31019476

RESUMO

In educational settings, tests are mainly used to measure the extent to which learners' knowledge and skill have been acquired. However, the act of taking a test also promotes learning itself. In particular, making errors on tests (i.e., searching for erroneous information) promotes learning. This is called the "failed retrieval effect" (FRE) and has been the subject of considerable study. Previous research shows that enhanced learning does not occur if feedback correcting an error is delayed. This is attributed to the relative absence of activated information. In this study, we manipulated both the amount of information to be retrieved prior to learning and the delay time until feedback is given to investigate their effects on learning. As a result, even when multiple incorrect answers were given to increase the degree of semantic activation, learning was not promoted beyond that found with traditional procedures that rely on only one incorrect response. The timing of feedback (immediate, short-delay, long-delay) also did not impact FRE. However, the manipulation of response format for erroneous information resulted in degraded performance when responses were typed and feedback was delayed. Based on this result, we suggested that the failed retrieval effect was not affected by semantic activation at the time of retrieval but was affected by response format. Moreover, the processing necessary for typing may affect FRE under the delayed feedback condition.

4.
Front Psychol ; 9: 672, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29896135

RESUMO

Accurately gauging the emotional experience of another person is important for navigating interpersonal interactions. This study investigated whether perceivers are capable of distinguishing between unintentionally expressed (genuine) and intentionally manipulated (posed) facial expressions attributed to four major emotions: amusement, disgust, sadness, and surprise. Sensitivity to this discrimination was explored by comparing unstaged dynamic and static facial stimuli and analyzing the results with signal detection theory. Participants indicated whether facial stimuli presented on a screen depicted a person showing a given emotion and whether that person was feeling a given emotion. The results showed that genuine displays were evaluated more as felt expressions than posed displays for all target emotions presented. In addition, sensitivity to the perception of emotional experience, or discriminability, was enhanced in dynamic facial displays, but was less pronounced in the case of static displays. This finding indicates that dynamic information in facial displays contributes to the ability to accurately infer the emotional experiences of another person.

5.
Behav Res Methods ; 50(4): 1415-1429, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29520632

RESUMO

Using appropriate stimuli to evoke emotions is especially important for researching emotion. Psychologists have provided several standardized affective stimulus databases-such as the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) and the Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS) as visual stimulus databases, as well as the International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) and the Montreal Affective Voices as auditory stimulus databases for emotional experiments. However, considering the limitations of the existing auditory stimulus database studies, research using auditory stimuli is relatively limited compared with the studies using visual stimuli. First, the number of sample sounds is limited, making it difficult to equate across emotional conditions and semantic categories. Second, some artificially created materials (music or human voice) may fail to accurately drive the intended emotional processes. Our principal aim was to expand existing auditory affective sample database to sufficiently cover natural sounds. We asked 207 participants to rate 935 sounds (including the sounds from the IADS-2) using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) and three basic-emotion rating scales. The results showed that emotions in sounds can be distinguished on the affective rating scales, and the stability of the evaluations of sounds revealed that we have successfully provided a larger corpus of natural, emotionally evocative auditory stimuli, covering a wide range of semantic categories. Our expanded, standardized sound sample database may promote a wide range of research in auditory systems and the possible interactions with other sensory modalities, encouraging direct reliable comparisons of outcomes from different researchers in the field of psychology.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Sintomas Afetivos , Bases de Dados Factuais/normas , Som , Adulto , Sintomas Afetivos/classificação , Sintomas Afetivos/diagnóstico , Escala de Avaliação Comportamental , Pesquisa Comportamental/métodos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Diferencial Semântico , Software
6.
Molecules ; 22(11)2017 Nov 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29113134

RESUMO

We prepared nanoscale, modularizable, self-assembled peptide nanoarchitectures with diameters less of than 20 nm by combining ß-sheet-forming peptides tethering a cell-penetrating peptide or a nuclear localization signal sequence. We also found that doxorubicin (Dox), an anti-cancer drug, was non-covalently accommodated by the assemblies at a ratio of one Dox molecule per ten peptides. The Dox-loaded peptide assemblies facilitated cellular uptake and subsequent nuclear localization in HeLa cells, and induced cell death even at low Dox concentrations. This peptide nanocarrier motif is a promising platform for a biocompatible drug delivery system by altering the targeting head groups of the carrier peptides.


Assuntos
Antibióticos Antineoplásicos/farmacologia , Peptídeos Penetradores de Células/química , Doxorrubicina/farmacologia , Sinais de Localização Nuclear/química , Antibióticos Antineoplásicos/química , Sobrevivência Celular/efeitos dos fármacos , Peptídeos Penetradores de Células/farmacologia , Doxorrubicina/química , Sistemas de Liberação de Medicamentos , Células HeLa , Humanos , Nanopartículas/química , Tamanho da Partícula
7.
Front Psychol ; 8: 890, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28626439

RESUMO

Recent studies have repeatedly demonstrated a false memory phenomenon in which people falsely remember having performed an action by oneself when in fact they have only observed the action by another person. We investigated the attentional effect to the action itself on the observation inflation. Fifty-four participants first performed and read actions (Phase 1); then, they observed the action video that showed another's actions (Phase 2), some of which they had not performed in Phase 1. In the Phase 2, they were required to focus on either the actor's performance (i.e., attentive observation condition) or irrelevant objects, which were presented in the background (i.e., inattentive observation condition) to modulate their attention. Around 2 weeks later, participants took a surprise source-memory test (Phase 3). In this phase, we asked them to judge whether they "performed," "read," or "not presented" the action in Phase 1. Three participants were removed from analysis, because they could not attend Phase 3 within 10-16 days after completion of the second phase. We found observation inflation only in the attentive condition, which contradicted the notions from other false memory studies that showed that attention to the target stimuli reduced false memory in general. We discussed the observation inflation mechanism from the perspective of the "like me" system, including the mirror neuron system, self-ownership, and self-agency.

8.
PLoS One ; 12(6): e0180041, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28662126

RESUMO

Most experimental studies of depressive symptom effects on decision-making have examined situations in which a single correct answer exists based on external circumstances (externally guided decision-making, e.g., gambling task). In addition to such decision-making, for decision-making of other types, no correct answer exists based on external circumstances (internally guided decision-making, e.g., preference judgment). For internally guided decision-making, a phenomenon is known by which preference for the chosen item increases and preference for the rejected item is decreased after choosing between two equally preferred items which is designated as choice-induced preference change. Recent reports suggest that this phenomenon is explainable by reinforcement learning theory just as it is with externally guided decision-making. Although many earlier studies have revealed the effects of depression in externally guided decision-making, the relation between depressive symptoms and choice-induced preference change remains unclear. This study investigated the relation between depressive symptoms and choice-induced preference change using the blind choice paradigm. Results show that depressive symptoms are correlated with change in preference of rejected items (Spearman's r = .28, p = .04): depressed individuals tend to show less decreased preference of rejected items. These results indicate that individual differences of depressive symptoms affect choice-induced preference change. We discuss the mechanisms underlying the relation between depression and choice-induced preference change.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Depressão/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
9.
Front Psychol ; 8: 633, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28522979

RESUMO

While numerous studies have examined the relationships between facial actions and emotions, they have yet to account for the ways that specific spontaneous facial expressions map onto emotional experiences induced without expressive intent. Moreover, previous studies emphasized that a fine-grained investigation of facial components could establish the coherence of facial actions with actual internal states. Therefore, this study aimed to accumulate evidence for the correspondence between spontaneous facial components and emotional experiences. We reinvestigated data from previous research which secretly recorded spontaneous facial expressions of Japanese participants as they watched film clips designed to evoke four different target emotions: surprise, amusement, disgust, and sadness. The participants rated their emotional experiences via a self-reported questionnaire of 16 emotions. These spontaneous facial expressions were coded using the Facial Action Coding System, the gold standard for classifying visible facial movements. We corroborated each facial action that was present in the emotional experiences by applying stepwise regression models. The results found that spontaneous facial components occurred in ways that cohere to their evolutionary functions based on the rating values of emotional experiences (e.g., the inner brow raiser might be involved in the evaluation of novelty). This study provided new empirical evidence for the correspondence between each spontaneous facial component and first-person internal states of emotion as reported by the expresser.

10.
Sci Rep ; 6: 32477, 2016 08 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27576670

RESUMO

Choosing an option increases a person's preference for that option. This phenomenon, called choice-based learning (CBL), has been investigated separately in the contexts of internally guided decision-making (IDM, e.g., preference judgment), for which no objectively correct answer exists, and externally guided decision making (EDM, e.g., perceptual decision making), for which one objectively correct answer exists. For the present study, we compared decision making of these two types to examine differences of underlying neural processes of CBL. As IDM and EDM tasks, occupation preference judgment and salary judgment were used, respectively. To compare CBL for the two types of decision making, we developed a novel measurement of CBL: decision consistency. When CBL occurs, decision consistency is higher in the last-half trials than in first-half trials. Electroencephalography (EEG) data have demonstrated that the change of decision consistency is positively correlated with the fronto-central beta-gamma power after response in the first-half trials for IDM, but not for EDM. Those results demonstrate for the first time the difference of CBL between IDM and EDM. The fronto-central beta-gamma power is expected to reflect a key process of CBL, specifically for IDM.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisão Clínica , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Gen Psychol ; 139(1): 29-41, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24836719

RESUMO

The self-reference effect (SRE), by which encoding of information is done in a self-referential manner (e.g., "Does the word describe you?"), enhances subsequent memory performance. It is thought to reflect that self-reference is a highly practiced task in everyday life. Accordingly, it is expected that the types of tasks that produce memory enhancement vary according to individual differences of past experiences. On the basis of neuroimaging studies, we hypothesized that social desirability reference ("Is this word socially desirable?") produces memory enhancement as with SRE in people who have chosen altruistic behavior frequently. Participants processed trait adjectives in relation to themselves, social desirability, and meaning. Then they performed a free recall task. The self-report altruism scale was used to assess the frequency of past altruistic behavior. Consistent with our prediction, the social desirability reference yielded the best retention in the high-altruism group. SRE was observed only in the low-altruism group.


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Memória , Autoimagem , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Testes Psicológicos , Desejabilidade Social , Adulto Jovem
12.
Shinrigaku Kenkyu ; 82(1): 56-62, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21706824

RESUMO

Elderly people have lower ability for recognizing facial emotions than younger people. Previous studies showed that older adults had difficulty in recognizing anger, sadness and fear, but there were no consistent results for happiness, surprise and disgust. Most of these studies used a small number of stimuli, and tabulated the number of correct responses for facial expressions. These characteristics of the task might be the source of the discrepancy in the findings. The present study used a task which measures participants' discrimination thresholds for six basic emotions using psychophysical measurement methods. The results showed that the thresholds for elderly participants (74.8 +/- 6.5 yrs) were significantly higher than for younger participants (20.1 +/- 1.6 yrs) for sadness, surprise, anger, disgust and fear. There was no significant difference for happiness. Since the task that we developed was sufficiently sensitive, it is a useful tool for assessing individuals' ability to perceive emotion.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica , Emoções , Expressão Facial , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Emoções Manifestas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Limiar Sensorial , Adulto Jovem
13.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 36(4): 455-69, 2010 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20363902

RESUMO

The authors investigated whether self-knowledge has a function to reduce conflict by biasing one of two choices during occupational choice (e.g., Which occupation do you think you could do better?-dancer or chemist). In the three experiments, event-related brain potentials were recorded. Experiment 1 revealed that the amplitude of the conflict-related negativity (CRN) reflects strength of conflict during occupational choice. Results of Experiment 2 demonstrated that the CRN amplitude during occupational choice was smaller when self-knowledge was activated than when other-knowledge was activated. Experiment 3 showed that the CRN amplitude during occupational choice was decreased more when self-knowledge that biases one choice of occupation was activated than when self-knowledge that does not bias was activated. These results suggest that self-knowledge reduces conflict by biasing one of multiple choices in situations where two or more possible correct answers can be given.


Assuntos
Escolha da Profissão , Conflito Psicológico , Preconceito , Autoimagem , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Japão , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Inquéritos e Questionários , Interface Usuário-Computador , Adulto Jovem
14.
Int J Neurosci ; 119(10): 1640-54, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19922379

RESUMO

We investigated whether the activation of self-knowledge reduces conflict during occupational choice, which can elicit many correct answers. Conflict was measured via event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Results of this study showed that activation of self-knowledge reduces conflict during occupational choice. Our results suggest that, in a situation in which two or more possible correct answers exist, people use self-knowledge as a benchmark to reduce conflict by biasing either choice of behavior.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Ocupações , Autoimagem , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
15.
Brain Inj ; 23(4): 291-8, 2009 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19330592

RESUMO

PRIMARY OBJECTIVE: Errorless learning has been reported to be effective in the rehabilitation of patients with impaired cognitive functions following brain injury. This study compared brain activations in errorless learning (EL) and errorful learning (EF) in patients with diffuse axonal injury (DAI) using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). METHODS AND PROCEDURES: The participants were 13 patients with DAI. Thirteen healthy individuals were evaluated as a control group. The participants learned words under the EL and EF conditions in advance and performed the recognition task during fMRI scanning. MAIN OUTCOMES AND RESULTS: EL in the control group was significantly faster than EF (p = 0.005), but not in the DAI group. EL in the DAI group scored significantly higher than EF (p = 0.026). An fMRI showed significant activations in the posterior cingulate gyrus (BA 31) and precuneus (BA 7) in the control group when EF > EL, but in the precuneus (BA 7, 31) and bilateral inferior parietal lobules (BA 39, 40) in the DAI group. CONCLUSIONS: These results indicate the disadvantage of EF and advantage of EL to DAI patients. The findings also reflect brain plasticity in patients with DAI in the chronic phase.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Lesão Axonal Difusa/fisiopatologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Transtornos Cognitivos/reabilitação , Lesão Axonal Difusa/reabilitação , Feminino , Humanos , Japão , Testes de Linguagem , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
16.
Psychol Rep ; 97(2): 515-26, 2005 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16342579

RESUMO

The 2001 conflict monitoring hypothesis of Botvinick and colleagues posits that the amount of conflict raised by incongruent stimuli in a flanker task affects subsequent cognitive control, such as response inhibition. The present experiment yielded empirical evidence of the quantitative relation between conflict and response inhibition. Participants judged the direction of a target arrow flanked by distractor arrows presented above and below the target. The amount of conflict was manipulated by varying the distance between the target and the directional distractors. Analysis showed that response times were longer for incongruent trials than for congruent trials, and response times on incongruent trials were longer for the small distance than for the large distance conditions. In addition, the response times in congruent trials became longer as the amount of conflict in the preceding trial increased. These results are consistent with Botvinick, et al.'s hypothesis that the conflict-detection mechanism determines the amount of response inhibition depending on the amount of conflict. Responses on incongruent trials were faster and more accurate when the preceding trial was incongruent than when it was congruent, and the size of this response facilitation was not influenced by the amount of conflict. These results suggest that the conflict detection mechanism modulates the subsequent behaviors by two forms of control which are differently affected by the amount of conflict.


Assuntos
Cognição , Inibição Psicológica , Testes Psicológicos , Tempo de Reação , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico , Adulto , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Comportamento Social
17.
Psychol Rep ; 96(3 Pt 2): 1055-65, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16173378

RESUMO

We investigated whether the desirability of trait adjectives was elaboratively processed during self-reference tasks (Exp. 1-1, 1-2) and whether desirability information played a role in memory processes in self-reference tasks (Exp. 2). In Exp. 1-1, 14 participants performed a series of initial tasks and target tasks, which were self-reference, evaluative, or semantic. Responses on the evaluating target task were faster when the initial task was self-reference rather than semantic. Exp. 1-2 demonstrated that information relevant to the evaluative task did not facilitate the process of performing an other-reference task. Although there was no evidence that desirability information had a role in the self-reference effect in Exp. 2, desirability of trait adjectives was specifically processed during self-reference tasks.


Assuntos
Autoimagem , Desejabilidade Social , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação
18.
Psychol Rep ; 97(3): 810-8, 2005 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16512299

RESUMO

We investigated whether affective integration increases the speed of processing of personality trait knowledge. The fan effect was compared between cases where trait knowledge is stored with the affective value and cases where it is not stored with the affective value. 18 college students first memorized a set of traits about fictitious individuals and then made recognition judgments. In the 2 x 2 factorial repeated-measures design, the number of traits learned about a fictitious individual and whether those traits were integrated by a shared affective value were manipulated. The significant interaction showed that knowledge of personality trait with affective integration was processed quickly even if the particular person's memory had rich connections with traits.


Assuntos
Afeto , Processos Mentais , Personalidade , Tempo de Reação , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudantes
19.
Neuroimage ; 21(4): 1604-11, 2004 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15050584

RESUMO

Neuroimaging data, particularly functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) findings, have not been reported in users of the myoelectric or electromyographic (EMG) prosthetic hand. We developed a virtual EMG prosthetic hand system to eliminate mutual signal noise interference between fMRI imaging and the EMG prosthesis. We used fMRI to localize activation in the human brain during manipulation of the virtual EMG prosthetic hand. Fourteen right-handed normal subjects were instructed to perform repetitive grasping with the right hand with eyes closed (CEG); repetitive grasping with the right hand with eyes open to obtain visual feedback of their own hand movement (OEG); and repetitive grasping with the virtual EMG prosthetic hand with the eyes open to obtain visual feedback of the prosthetic hand movement (VRG). The specific site activated during manipulation of the EMG prosthetic hand was the right ventral premotor cortex. Both paradigms with visual feedback also (OEG and VRG) demonstrated activation in the right posterior parietal cortex. The center of activation of the right posterior parietal cortex shifted laterally for visual feedback with the virtual EMG prosthetic hand compared to a subject's own hand. The results suggest that the EMG prosthetic hand might be recognized in the brain as a high-performance alternative to a real hand, being controlled through a "mirror system" in the brain.


Assuntos
Membros Artificiais , Eletromiografia/instrumentação , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Mãos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/instrumentação , Imageamento Tridimensional/instrumentação , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/instrumentação , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Interface Usuário-Computador , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Biorretroalimentação Psicológica/instrumentação , Biorretroalimentação Psicológica/fisiologia , Cerebelo/fisiologia , Gráficos por Computador , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Eletrodos , Processamento Eletrônico de Dados/instrumentação , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/inervação , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Oxigênio/sangue , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador/instrumentação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...