Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Vision Res ; 145: 1-10, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29608936

RESUMO

In human visual processing, information from the visual field passes through numerous transformations before perceptual attributes such as colour are derived. The sequence of transforms involved in constructing perceptions of colour can be approximated by colour appearance models such as the CIE (2002) colour appearance model, abbreviated as CIECAM02. In this study, we test the plausibility of CIECAM02 as a model of colour processing by looking for evidence of its cortical entrainment. The CIECAM02 model predicts that colour is split in to two opposing chromatic components, red-green and cyan-yellow (termed CIECAM02-a and CIECAM02-b respectively), and an achromatic component (termed CIECAM02-A). Entrainment of cortical activity to the outputs of these components was estimated using measurements of electro- and magnetoencephalographic (EMEG) activity, recorded while healthy subjects watched videos of dots changing colour. We find entrainment to chromatic component CIECAM02-a at approximately 35 ms latency bilaterally in occipital lobe regions, and entrainment to achromatic component CIECAM02-A at approximately 75 ms latency, also bilaterally in occipital regions. For comparison, transforms from a less physiologically plausible model (CIELAB) were also tested, with no significant entrainment found.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Potenciais Evocados Visuais , Feminino , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Adulto Jovem
2.
J Comput Neurosci ; 43(1): 1-4, 2017 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28643213

RESUMO

Describing the human brain in mathematical terms is an important ambition of neuroscience research, yet the challenges remain considerable. It was Alan Turing, writing in 1950, who first sought to demonstrate how time-consuming such an undertaking would be. Through analogy to the computer program, Turing argued that arriving at a complete mathematical description of the mind would take well over a thousand years. In this opinion piece, we argue that - despite seventy years of progress in the field - his arguments remain both prescient and persuasive.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Matemática , Modelos Neurológicos , Humanos
3.
Hear Res ; 344: 244-254, 2017 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27915027

RESUMO

A prominent feature of the auditory system is that neurons show tuning to audio frequency; each neuron has a characteristic frequency (CF) to which it is most sensitive. Furthermore, there is an orderly mapping of CF to position, which is called tonotopic organization and which is observed at many levels of the auditory system. In a previous study (Thwaites et al., 2016) we examined cortical entrainment to two auditory transforms predicted by a model of loudness, instantaneous loudness and short-term loudness, using speech as the input signal. The model is based on the assumption that neural activity is combined across CFs (i.e. across frequency channels) before the transform to short-term loudness. However, it is also possible that short-term loudness is determined on a channel-specific basis. Here we tested these possibilities by assessing neural entrainment to the overall and channel-specific instantaneous loudness and the overall and channel-specific short-term loudness. The results showed entrainment to channel-specific instantaneous loudness at latencies of 45 and 100 ms (bilaterally, in and around Heschl's gyrus). There was entrainment to overall instantaneous loudness at 165 ms in dorso-lateral sulcus (DLS). Entrainment to overall short-term loudness occurred primarily at 275 ms, bilaterally in DLS and superior temporal sulcus. There was only weak evidence for entrainment to channel-specific short-term loudness.


Assuntos
Vias Auditivas/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Audição , Percepção Sonora , Magnetoencefalografia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Audiometria da Fala , Vias Auditivas/citologia , Córtex Cerebral/citologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Tempo de Reação , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
4.
Front Neurosci ; 10: 183, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27199645

RESUMO

Acoustic signals pass through numerous transforms in the auditory system before perceptual attributes such as loudness and pitch are derived. However, relatively little is known as to exactly when these transformations happen, and where, cortically or sub-cortically, they occur. In an effort to examine this, we investigated the latencies and locations of cortical entrainment to two transforms predicted by a model of loudness perception for time-varying sounds: the transforms were instantaneous loudness and short-term loudness, where the latter is hypothesized to be derived from the former and therefore should occur later in time. Entrainment of cortical activity was estimated from electro- and magneto-encephalographic (EMEG) activity, recorded while healthy subjects listened to continuous speech. There was entrainment to instantaneous loudness bilaterally at 45, 100, and 165 ms, in Heschl's gyrus, dorsal lateral sulcus, and Heschl's gyrus, respectively. Entrainment to short-term loudness was found in both the dorsal lateral sulcus and superior temporal sulcus at 275 ms. These results suggest that short-term loudness is derived from instantaneous loudness, and that this derivation occurs after processing in sub-cortical structures.

5.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25713530

RESUMO

A primary objective for cognitive neuroscience is to identify how features of the sensory environment are encoded in neural activity. Current auditory models of loudness perception can be used to make detailed predictions about the neural activity of the cortex as an individual listens to speech. We used two such models (loudness-sones and loudness-phons), varying in their psychophysiological realism, to predict the instantaneous loudness contours produced by 480 isolated words. These two sets of 480 contours were used to search for electrophysiological evidence of loudness processing in whole-brain recordings of electro- and magneto-encephalographic (EMEG) activity, recorded while subjects listened to the words. The technique identified a bilateral sequence of loudness processes, predicted by the more realistic loudness-sones model, that begin in auditory cortex at ~80 ms and subsequently reappear, tracking progressively down the superior temporal sulcus (STS) at lags from 230 to 330 ms. The technique was then extended to search for regions sensitive to the fundamental frequency (F0) of the voiced parts of the speech. It identified a bilateral F0 process in auditory cortex at a lag of ~90 ms, which was not followed by activity in STS. The results suggest that loudness information is being used to guide the analysis of the speech stream as it proceeds beyond auditory cortex down STS toward the temporal pole.

6.
Front Neuroinform ; 8: 8, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24600385

RESUMO

Diffusion Imaging in Python (Dipy) is a free and open source software project for the analysis of data from diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (dMRI) experiments. dMRI is an application of MRI that can be used to measure structural features of brain white matter. Many methods have been developed to use dMRI data to model the local configuration of white matter nerve fiber bundles and infer the trajectory of bundles connecting different parts of the brain. Dipy gathers implementations of many different methods in dMRI, including: diffusion signal pre-processing; reconstruction of diffusion distributions in individual voxels; fiber tractography and fiber track post-processing, analysis and visualization. Dipy aims to provide transparent implementations for all the different steps of dMRI analysis with a uniform programming interface. We have implemented classical signal reconstruction techniques, such as the diffusion tensor model and deterministic fiber tractography. In addition, cutting edge novel reconstruction techniques are implemented, such as constrained spherical deconvolution and diffusion spectrum imaging (DSI) with deconvolution, as well as methods for probabilistic tracking and original methods for tractography clustering. Many additional utility functions are provided to calculate various statistics, informative visualizations, as well as file-handling routines to assist in the development and use of novel techniques. In contrast to many other scientific software projects, Dipy is not being developed by a single research group. Rather, it is an open project that encourages contributions from any scientist/developer through GitHub and open discussions on the project mailing list. Consequently, Dipy today has an international team of contributors, spanning seven different academic institutions in five countries and three continents, which is still growing.

7.
Front Neurosci ; 6: 175, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23248578

RESUMO

Diffusion MR data sets produce large numbers of streamlines which are hard to visualize, interact with, and interpret in a clinically acceptable time scale, despite numerous proposed approaches. As a solution we present a simple, compact, tailor-made clustering algorithm, QuickBundles (QB), that overcomes the complexity of these large data sets and provides informative clusters in seconds. Each QB cluster can be represented by a single centroid streamline; collectively these centroid streamlines can be taken as an effective representation of the tractography. We provide a number of tests to show how the QB reduction has good consistency and robustness. We show how the QB reduction can help in the search for similarities across several subjects.

8.
PLoS One ; 7(11): e48789, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23144970

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Previous research suggested that structural and functional abnormalities within the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex contribute to the pathophysiology of Conduct Disorder (CD). Here, we investigated whether the integrity of the white-matter pathways connecting these regions is abnormal and thus may represent a putative neurobiological marker for CD. METHODS: Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) was used to investigate white-matter microstructural integrity in male adolescents with childhood-onset CD, compared with healthy controls matched in age, sex, intelligence, and socioeconomic status. Two approaches were employed to analyze DTI data: voxel-based morphometry of fractional anisotropy (FA), an index of white-matter integrity, and virtual dissection of white-matter pathways using tractography. RESULTS: Adolescents with CD displayed higher FA within the right external capsule relative to controls (T = 6.08, P<0.05, Family-Wise Error, whole-brain correction). Tractography analyses showed that FA values within the uncinate fascicle (connecting the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex) were abnormally increased in individuals with CD relative to controls. This was in contrast with the inferior frontal-occipital fascicle, which showed no significant group differences in FA. The finding of increased FA in the uncinate fascicle remained significant when factoring out the contribution of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. There were no group differences in the number of streamlines in either of these anatomical tracts. CONCLUSIONS: These results provide evidence that CD is associated with white-matter microstructural abnormalities in the anatomical tract that connects the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, the uncinate fascicle. These results implicate abnormal maturation of white-matter pathways which are fundamental in the regulation of emotional behavior in CD.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/patologia , Transtorno da Conduta/patologia , Lobo Frontal/patologia , Adolescente , Anisotropia , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(33): 14899-902, 2010 Aug 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20679241

RESUMO

Tests of fluid intelligence predict success in a wide range of cognitive activities. Much uncertainty has surrounded brain lesions producing deficits in these tests, with standard group comparisons delivering no clear result. Based on findings from functional imaging, we propose that the uncertainty of lesion data may arise from the specificity and complexity of the relevant neural circuit. Fluid intelligence tests give a characteristic pattern of activity in posterolateral frontal, dorsomedial frontal, and midparietal cortex. To test the causal role of these regions, we examined fluid intelligence in 80 patients with focal cortical lesions. Damage to each of the proposed regions predicted fluid intelligence loss, whereas damage outside these regions was not predictive. The results suggest that coarse group comparisons (e.g., frontal vs. posterior) cannot show the neural underpinnings of fluid intelligence tests. Instead, deficits reflect the extent of damage to a restricted but complex brain circuit comprising specific regions within both frontal and posterior cortex.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiopatologia , Inteligência , Rede Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Idoso , Mapeamento Encefálico , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/patologia , Humanos , Testes de Inteligência , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Rede Nervosa/patologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Lobo Parietal/patologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Análise de Regressão , Adulto Jovem
10.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 30(12): 3837-50, 2009 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19554560

RESUMO

Here, we ask whether frontotemporal cortex is functionally dissociated into distributed lexical and category-specific semantic networks. To this end, fMRI activation patterns elicited during the processing of words from different semantic categories were categorized using k-means cluster algorithms. Results showed a distributed pattern of inferiorfrontal, superiortemporal, and fusiform activation shared by different word categories. This shared activation contrasted with patterns of category-specific semantic activation in widely distributed neural systems. Clustering revealed congruent functional specificity of focal area activations in frontal and temporal cortex; thus suggesting a correspondence between functional partitionings of frontocentral mirror neuron systems and those of inferiortemporal lexical and semantic circuits. Action words related to the face, arms, and legs specifically activated the motor system in a somatotopic manner, whereas form-related words activated prefrontal areas. Similar functional specificity was evident in temporal cortex, where a different semantic topography emerged for form- and action-related words. Results were replicated in a separate data set, therefore recommending fMRI cluster analysis as a reliable method for scrutinizing the brain basis of lexical, semantic, and conceptual systems in humans. As focal modules do not explain the distributed character of functionally specific clusters and their distinct topographies are at variance with general distributed processing accounts, the functionally-homogenous distributed clusters specific to semantic types are best explained by specifically-distributed cortical circuits which, similar to Hebbian cell assemblies, represent functional units with specific roles in cognitive processing, especially in lexical and semantic access and memory.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Semântica , Adulto , Análise por Conglomerados , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 105(33): 11969-74, 2008 Aug 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18689686

RESUMO

The frontal lobes play a key role in sequential organization of behavior. Little is known, however, of the way frontal neurons code successive phases of a structured task plan. Using correlational analysis, we asked how a population of frontal cells represents the multiple events of a complex sequential task. Monkeys performed a conventional cue-target association task, with distinct cue, delay, and target phases. Across the population of recorded cells, we examined patterns of activity for different task phases, and in the same phase, for different stimulus objects. The results show hierarchical representation of task events. For different task phases, there were different, approximately orthogonal patterns of activity across the population of neurons. Modulations of each basic pattern encoded stimulus information within each phase. By orthogonal coding, the frontal lobe may control transitions between the discrete steps of a mental program; by correlated coding within each step, similar operations may be applied to different stimulus content.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Animais , Análise por Conglomerados
12.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 137(1): 131-48, 2008 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18248133

RESUMO

In goal neglect, a person ignores some task requirement though being able to describe it. Goal neglect is closely related to general intelligence or C. Spearman's (1904) g (J. Duncan, H. Emslie, P. Williams, R. Johnson, & C. Freer, 1996). The authors tested the role of task complexity in neglect and the hypothesis that different task components in some sense compete for attention. In contrast to many kinds of attentional limits, increasing the real-time demands of one task component does not promote neglect of another. Neither does neglect depend on preparation for different possible events in a block of trials. Instead, the key factor is complexity in the whole body of knowledge specified in task instructions. The authors suggest that as novel activity is constructed, relevant facts, rules, and requirements must be organized into a "task model." As this model increases in complexity, different task components compete for representation, and vulnerable components may be lost. Construction of effective task models is closely linked to g.


Assuntos
Atenção , Objetivos , Psicologia/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Humanos , Inteligência , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(7): 1222-9, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16490222

RESUMO

Although damage to right posterior parietal cortex (RPPC) produces bias in line bisection, Karnath et al. [Karnath, H.-O., Berger, M. F., Küker, W., & Rorden, C. (2004). The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: A study of 140 patients. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1164-1172] claim that it plays little role in spatial neglect, which is better measured by target cancellation. We used a detection task (approximating cancellation in requiring detection) to investigate this claim by compromising the parietal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Two outline shapes, one on each side of fixation, were briefly displayed before a mask. The target was a discontinuity in the left or right of the outline of one of these perceptual objects. Subjects indicated position or absence of target as fast as possible. Stimulus-mask onset asynchrony was adjusted individually to yield 75% detection. TMS was delivered over left posterior parietal cortex (LPPC), RPPC and Vertex, with Sham TMS over RPPC as a baseline control. Target detection was near ceiling and fastest at central positions and worst and slowest at the far right. Detection was significantly reduced at the far left position by TMS over RPPC. No other effects were obtained and latency was not affected by TMS. Disruption of RPPC by TMS does produce left neglect as measured by detection. Given the pattern of performance and since it was disrupted on one side of the display rather than on one side of each shape, attention and neglect were in a scene-based rather than object-based reference frame.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Área de Dependência-Independência , Orientação/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fechamento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo
14.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(5): 693-710, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16239017

RESUMO

A previous paper reported high susceptibility to spatial migration (allochiria) of tactile stimuli in about 25% of healthy individuals (High Error subjects). When synchronous stimuli touched the two hands, if the unattended stimulus was temporally modulated when the attended one was not (and was thus more salient than the latter), it "migrated" to and fused with or replaced the stimulus on the attended hand. When subjects rated similarity of the attended stimulus accompanied by a distractor to each stimulus alone, scaling distributions tested against a sampling model showed most High Error subjects experienced fused stimuli, others experienced replacement and Low Error subjects experienced neither. We argued that these migrations are equivalent to allochiria and that this underlies neglect and extinction. This study assessed whether the individual difference is modality-specific or not. In auditory and visual equivalents of the tactile rating experiment, the difference between High and Low Error subjects was replicated in audition, but no migration occurred in vision. However, when two words were briefly presented visually before a mask with cued report of one, letter migrations to equivalent locations did occur and the individual difference was reproduced. This constitutes the first report of individual differences in auditory fusion and visual letter migration. Migration occurred in egocentric coordinates but apparently preserved structural homology. Different migration rates between the modalities paralleled relative salience of the unattended to the attended stimulus. The multimodality of the individual difference suggests that its source is supramodal, in deficient binding of perceptual content to location.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Sensação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Suscetibilidade a Doenças , Relação Dose-Resposta à Radiação , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Física/métodos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
15.
Ear Hear ; 26(3): 251-62, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15937407

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To develop and implement a new audiological fitting procedure for auditory brain stem implants (ABIs), based on an efficient algorithm, and to compare it with two procedures presently used in clinical practice. DESIGN: First, the different procedures were compared by using computer models and simulations with normal-hearing subjects (N = 4). This allows for an analysis of the accuracy of the procedures in a way that is not possible when testing ABI users. The root-mean-square error between the order estimated by the procedure and the true order was calculated. In addition, ABI users (N = 2) were tested with the new procedure to see if it could be successfully applied in clinic. The degree of variability of their results across runs and sessions was analyzed. RESULTS: The tests of the normal-hearing subjects showed that our proposed procedure required significantly fewer trials (22 on average) than procedures presently used in clinic (with 76 and 234 trials on average for the two other procedures tested) to produce the same degree of accuracy. Computer modeling also demonstrated this advantage. Additional testing showed this advantage was maintained under a variety of conditions relevant to the clinic. The two patients tested were able to use this procedure with success, even though they were poor at discriminating the pitch of electrodes. The patients showed results consistent with having about 4 to 5 discriminable groups of electrodes with the 12 to 14 electrodes tested. CONCLUSIONS: The proposed procedure requires fewer trials to produce a clinically useful result and is well tolerated in the clinic. An additional advantage is that it allows testing to be broken down into several "blocks," each containing a small number of trials. If the variability between blocks is small, information can be combined across blocks to increase the accuracy of the result. If the variability is large, perhaps between blocks on different days, this may reflect a significant change in the percepts generated by the implant, and signal to the clinician that a significant alteration in the fitting is required. We recommend its use in ABI user fitting and in cochlear implant fitting when pitch ranking is problematic.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Implantes Auditivos de Tronco Encefálico , Simulação por Computador , Perda Auditiva Bilateral/reabilitação , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Desenho de Equipamento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ajuste de Prótese
16.
Memory ; 12(5): 655-70, 2004 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15615322

RESUMO

Depression and dysphoric mood states are often accompanied by quantitative or qualitative shifts in performance across a range of retention tasks. This study focuses on the recollection of both autobiographical events and word lists in dysphoric states. Recollection occurs when people are aware of some contextual detail allied to the encoding experience. This study establishes the presence of a recollection deficit in dysphoria in two distinct paradigms. In both autobiographical recall and in recognition memory, recollection in a dysphoric group was at lower levels than recollection in matched controls. The study examines the hypothesis that the extent of recollection is influenced by two factors: (1) the degree of differentiation of schematic mental models; and (2) the executive mode that predominates when memory tasks are carried out, with the latter assumed to be altered by rumination. The relationship between responses based on recollection and alternative mnemonic responses could be predicted by measures of these two factors. The results are discussed in terms of the Interacting Cognitive Subsystems model (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993) and the perspective it offers on the relationship between meaning systems and executive functions (Barnard, 1999).


Assuntos
Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Rememoração Mental , Transtornos do Humor/psicologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Cognição , Sinais (Psicologia) , Depressão/complicações , Depressão/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Modelos Psicológicos , Transtornos do Humor/complicações , Testes Psicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Análise de Regressão , Retenção Psicológica , Inquéritos e Questionários
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 42(13): 1749-67, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15351625

RESUMO

Migration of tactile sensation was found to occur very frequently in about 25% of normal people (High Error subjects) and very infrequently in others. When synchronous stimuli touched the two hands, if the unattended stimulus was modulated when the attended one was not (and was thus more salient) it "migrated" to the attended hand and fused with or replaced the attended stimulus. However, latencies reflecting congruence and incongruence of simultaneous stimuli showed that their identities on each hand had been (nonconsciously) registered veridically. Subsequent experiments, involving Focused and Divided Attention without speed pressure showed that mislocation errors in these subjects (i) were not due to confusion about location of otherwise perceptually distinct stimuli, (ii) nor to speed demand, (iii) nor to relative salience per se, (iv) were immune to attentional manipulation and practice in most subjects, (v) required a stimulus on the attended hand, and (vi) reflected a changed experience. Finally the same subjects rated similarity of the attended stimulus when accompanied by a distractor to each stimulus alone. Scaling distributions tested against a sampling model showed that most High Error subjects experienced a fusion of the stimuli, some experienced a replacement, and Low Error subjects experienced neither. The individual difference appears to be in attentional separability and spatial binding of tactile stimuli. Mislocation to the focus of spatial attention is common in healthy people, especially when binding is prevented. The present phenomenon appears equivalent to allochiria, but also accounts for phenomena in neglect and extinction, and suggests a premorbid susceptibility to spatial migration and integration that can be exaggerated by brain damage.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção/fisiologia , Sensação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Idoso , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Suscetibilidade a Doenças , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Física/métodos , Tempo de Reação , Fatores de Tempo
18.
Cortex ; 40(1): 19-40, 2004 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15070001

RESUMO

This study of anosognosia for hemiplegia investigated: whether it is homogeneous; specificity to plegia of unawareness; extension to different kinds of and objects of awareness regarding plegia; partiality of unawareness. Sixty-four hemiplegic stroke patients were assessed with control subjects on (a) motor and somatosensory function, immediately followed by participants' evaluations of performance; (b) conventional structured interview questions addressing awareness of various capacities: (c) Neglect, Mental Flexibility, General Mental State, Verbal Fluency, Short-Term Memory; (d) pre- and post-performance estimates of ability on the last two; (e) estimates of current ability on bilateral and unilateral tasks, addressed by questions in 1st- and 3rd-person forms, explanations of how overestimated tasks would be accomplished, attempts at 3 bimanual tasks and post-attempt estimates of ability on these. Anosognosia for plegia was mostly associated with right-brain damage. No single factor or combination accounted for all patients. Double dissociations indicated that anosognosia can be specific to plegia: and patients do not generally overestimate other abilities. Although unawareness of paralysis and of its consequences appear linked, the latter is more widespread and persistent. Double dissociation showed that concurrent unawareness of movement failures is a separate deficit from these. There was differential awareness of different aspects of plegia. Further, some patients who overestimated current bilateral task ability when asked in 1st-person form did not overestimate when asked how well the examiner, if he was in their current condition, could do each task. This suggests split awareness of a single aspect of plegia. Patients anosognosic on conventional questioning showed two distinctions. (1) Some were unaware of movement failures when they occurred; others were aware but quickly forgot such failures and seem unable to update long-term body knowledge. (2) Some patients' explanations of bimanual task performance reflect unawareness of hemiplegia; others' explanations were bizarre and imply some awareness. The latter group's deficit appears to be nonspecific and linked to right-hemisphere predominance of anosognosia, an account of which is offered. Anosognosia for hemiplegia is not a unitary phenomenon: several factors underlie deficits in bodily awareness.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Negação em Psicologia , Hemiplegia/fisiopatologia , Hemiplegia/psicologia , Autoimagem , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/psicologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Dano Encefálico Crônico/fisiopatologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/psicologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Cultura , Delusões , Transtornos Dissociativos/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Dissociativos/psicologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Hemiplegia/etiologia , Humanos , Análise por Pareamento , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transtornos da Percepção/etiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Valores de Referência , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações , Fatores de Tempo , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia
19.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 3(3): 207-33, 2003 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14672157

RESUMO

The application of functional neuroimaging to the study of human emotion has yielded valuable data; however, the conclusions that may be drawn from any one study are limited. We applied novel statistical techniques to the meta-analysis of 106 PET and fMRI studies of human emotion and tested predictions made by key neuroscientific models. The results demonstrated partial support for asymmetry accounts. Greater left-sided activity was observed for approach emotions, whereas neural activity associated with negative/withdrawal emotions was symmetrical. Support was also found for affect program emotion accounts. The activation distributions associated with fear, disgust, and anger differed significantly. These emotions were most consistently associated in activity in regions associated with selective processing deficits when damaged: the amygdala, the insula and globus pallidus, and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, respectively. In contrast, the distributions for happiness and sadness did not differ. These findings are considered in the context of conceptualizations of the neural correlates of human emotion.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Neuroanatomia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Dominância Cerebral , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Radioisótopos de Oxigênio , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão
20.
Neuropsychologia ; 41(2): 195-202, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12459217

RESUMO

We report three experiments investigating the recognition of emotion from facial expressions across the adult life span. Increasing age produced a progressive reduction in the recognition of fear and, to a lesser extent, anger. In contrast, older participants showed no reduction in recognition of disgust, rather there was some evidence of an improvement. The results are discussed in terms of studies from the neuropsychological and functional imaging literature that indicate that separate brain regions may underlie the emotions fear and disgust. We suggest that the dissociable effects found for fear and disgust are consistent with the differential effects of ageing on brain regions involved in these emotions.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Ira/fisiologia , Gânglios da Base/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...