Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 54
Filtrar
1.
Heliyon ; 10(6): e27483, 2024 Mar 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38496889

RESUMO

When a pedestrian intends to cross the street, it is essential for safe mobility to correctly estimate the arrival time (time-to-collision, TTC) of an approaching vehicle. However, visual perception of acceleration is rather imprecise. Previous studies consistently showed that humans (mostly) disregard acceleration, but judge the TTC for an object as if it were traveling at constant speed (first-order estimation), which is associated with overestimated TTCs for positively accelerating objects. In a traffic context, such TTC overestimation could motivate pedestrians to cross in front of an approaching vehicle, although the time remaining is not sufficiently long. Can a simple acceleration signal help improve visual TTC estimation for accelerating objects? The present study investigated whether a signal that only indicates whether a vehicle is accelerating or not can remove the first-order pattern of overestimated TTCs. In a virtual reality simulation, 26 participants estimated the TTC of vehicles that approached with constant velocity or accelerated, from the perspective of a pedestrian at the curb. In half of the experimental blocks, a light band on the windshield illuminated whenever the vehicle accelerated but remained deactivated when the vehicle travelled at a constant speed. In the other blocks, the light band never illuminated, regardless of whether or not the vehicle accelerated. Participants were informed about the light band function in each block. Without acceleration signal, the estimated TTCs for the accelerating vehicles were consistent with an erroneous first-order approximation. In blocks with acceleration signal, participants substantially changed their estimation strategy, so that TTC overestimations for accelerating vehicles were reduced. Our data suggest that a binary acceleration signal helps pedestrians to effectively reduce the TTC overestimation for accelerating vehicles and could therefore increase pedestrian safety.

2.
Br J Psychol ; 115(2): 275-305, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38041610

RESUMO

As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour-emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour-emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour.


Assuntos
Afeto , Emoções , Adolescente , Humanos , Idoso , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cor , Percepção de Cores , Nível de Alerta
3.
PLoS One ; 18(8): e0288206, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37531322

RESUMO

When judging the time-to-collision (TTC) of visually presented accelerating vehicles, untrained observers do not adequately account for acceleration (second-order information). Instead, their estimations only rely on vehicle distance and velocity (first-order information). As a result, they systemically overestimate the TTC for accelerating objects, which represents a potential risk for pedestrians in traffic situations because it might trigger unsafe road-crossing behavior. Can training help reduce these estimation errors? In this study, we tested whether training with trial-by-trial feedback about the signed deviation of the estimated from the actual TTC can improve TTC estimation accuracy for accelerating vehicles. Using a prediction-motion paradigm, we measured the estimated TTCs of twenty participants for constant-velocity and accelerated vehicle approaches, from a pedestrian's perspective in a VR traffic simulation. The experiment included three blocks, of which only the second block provided trial-by-trial feedback about the TTC estimation accuracy. Participants adjusted their estimations during and after the feedback, but they failed to differentiate between accelerated and constant-velocity approaches. Thus, the feedback did not help them account for acceleration. The results suggest that a safety training program based on trial-by-trial feedback is not a promising countermeasure against pedestrians' erroneous TTC estimation for accelerating objects.


Assuntos
Acidentes de Trânsito , Pedestres , Humanos , Aceleração , Acidentes de Trânsito/prevenção & controle , Simulação por Computador , Retroalimentação , Segurança
4.
Accid Anal Prev ; 175: 106778, 2022 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35878469

RESUMO

To avoid collision, pedestrians intending to cross a road need to estimate the time-to-collision (TTC) of an approaching vehicle. Here, we present a novel interactive audiovisual virtual-reality system for investigating how the acoustic characteristics (loudness and engine type) of vehicles influence the TTC estimation. Using acoustic recordings of real vehicles as source signals, the dynamic spatial sound fields corresponding to a vehicle approaching in an urban setting are generated based on physical modeling of the sound propagation between vehicle and pedestrian and are presented via sound field synthesis. We studied TTC estimation for vehicles with internal combustion engine and for loudness-matched electric vehicles. The vehicle sound levels were varied by 10 dB, independently of the speed, presented TTC, and vehicle type. In an auditory-only condition, the cars were not visible, and lower loudness of the cars resulted in considerably longer TTC estimates. Importantly, the loudness of the cars also had a significant effect in the same direction on the TTC estimates in an audiovisual condition, where the cars were additionally visually presented via interactive virtual-reality simulations. Thus, pedestrians use auditory information when estimating TTC, even when full visual information is available. At equal loudness, the TTC judgments for electric and conventional vehicles were virtually identical, indicating that loudness has a stronger effect than spectral differences. Because TTC overestimations can result in risky road crossing decisions, the results imply that vehicle loudness should be considered as an important factor in pedestrian safety.


Assuntos
Pedestres , Realidade Virtual , Acidentes de Trânsito , Automóveis , Humanos , Segurança , Som , Caminhada
5.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 1863, 2022 02 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35115600

RESUMO

With the Covid-19 pandemic, many governments introduced nationwide lockdowns that disrupted people's daily routines and promoted social isolation. We applied a longitudinal online survey to investigate the mid-term effects of the mandated restrictions on the perceived passage of time (PPT) and boredom during and after a strict lockdown in Germany. One week after the beginning of the lockdown in March 2020, respondents reported a slower PPT and increased boredom compared to the pre-pandemic level. However, in the course of the lockdown, PPT accelerated and boredom decreased again until August 2020. Then, in October 2020, when incidence rates sharply rose and new restrictions were introduced, we again observed a slight trend toward a slowing of PPT and an increase of boredom. Our data also show that as the pandemic progressed, respondents adjusted their predictions about the pandemic's duration substantially upward. In sum, our findings suggest that respondents adapted to the pandemic situation and anticipated it as the new "normal". Furthermore, we determined perceived boredom and the general emotional state to be predictive of PPT, while depressive symptoms played a minor role.


Assuntos
Tédio , COVID-19/epidemiologia , COVID-19/prevenção & controle , Controle de Doenças Transmissíveis/métodos , Estudos Longitudinais , Isolamento Social/psicologia , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Percepção do Tempo , Adaptação Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , COVID-19/psicologia , Feminino , Alemanha/epidemiologia , Humanos , Incidência , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
6.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0261001, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34941913

RESUMO

Loudness judgments of sounds varying in level across time show a non-uniform temporal weighting, with increased weights assigned to the beginning of the sound (primacy effect). In addition, higher weights are observed for temporal components that are higher in level than the remaining components (loudness dominance). In three experiments, sounds consisting of 100- or 475-ms Gaussian wideband noise segments with random level variations were presented and either none, the first, or a central temporal segment was amplified or attenuated. In Experiment 1, the sounds consisted of four 100-ms segments that were separated by 500-ms gaps. Previous experiments did not show a primacy effect in such a condition. In Experiment 2, four- or ten-100-ms-segment sounds without gaps between the segments were presented to examine the interaction between the primacy effect and level dominance. As expected, for the sounds with segments separated by gaps, no primacy effect was observed, but weights on amplified segments were increased and weights on attenuated segments were decreased. For the sounds with contiguous segments, a primacy effect as well as effects of relative level (similar to those in Experiment 1) were found. For attenuation, the data indicated no substantial interaction between the primacy effect and loudness dominance, whereas for amplification an interaction was present. In Experiment 3, sounds consisting of either four contiguous 100-ms or 475-ms segments, or four 100-ms segments separated by 500-ms gaps were presented. Effects of relative level were more pronounced for the contiguous sounds. Across all three experiments, the effects of relative level were more pronounced for attenuation. In addition, the effects of relative level showed a dependence on the position of the change in level, with opposite direction for attenuation compared to amplification. Some of the results are in accordance with explanations based on masking effects on auditory intensity resolution.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção Sonora , Psicoacústica , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Ruído , Som , Adulto Jovem
7.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 47(8): 1132-1148, 2021 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34516218

RESUMO

We conducted two experiments to investigate how observers integrate postural and visual eye-height information when estimating the layout of interior space. In Experiment 1, we varied postural and visual eye-height information independently of each other in a virtual-reality setup. Observers estimated the width, depth, and height of simulated rooms. All dimensions were perceived as larger when the virtual visual eye-height corresponded to sitting on the floor as compared with standing upright. In contrast, the estimates remained widely unaffected by the observer's physical posture (likewise sitting vs. standing). In Experiment 2, we studied effects of the viewing condition (real vs. virtual rooms) and (in case of the virtual rooms) adaptation to congruence versus incongruence of visual and postural information. Both media yielded comparable results, which indicates that eye-height information is processed similarly in virtual and genuine reality. In addition, observers adapted to the (in)congruence of visual and postural cues. When we presented trials with congruent information first, both visual and postural cues had an effect on the estimates. However, when information was initially incongruent, observers mostly relied on visual cues, presumably relative to an internalized standard, and disregarded postural cues. Taken together, our results show that the integration of visual and postural eye-height information is situation-dependent. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Realidade Virtual , Humanos , Percepção , Equilíbrio Postural , Postura , Percepção Visual
8.
Front Psychol ; 12: 588571, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33815193

RESUMO

Previous work showed that the beginning of a sound is more important for the perception of loudness than later parts. When a short silent gap of sufficient duration is inserted into a sound, this primacy effect reoccurs in the second sound part after the gap. The present study investigates whether this temporal weighting occurs independently for different frequency bands. Sounds consisting of two bandpass noises were presented in four different conditions: (1) a simultaneous gap in both bands, (2) a gap in only the lower frequency band, (3) a gap in only the higher frequency band, or (4) no gap. In all conditions, the temporal loudness weights showed a primacy effect at sound onset. For the frequency bands without a gap, the temporal weights decreased gradually across time, regardless of whether the other frequency band did or did not contain a gap. When a frequency band contained a gap, the weight at the onset of this band after the gap was increased. This reoccurrence of the primacy effect following the gap was again largely independent of whether or not the other band contained a gap. Thus, the results indicate that the temporal loudness weights are frequency specific.

9.
Psychol Sci ; 31(10): 1245-1260, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32900287

RESUMO

Many of us "see red," "feel blue," or "turn green with envy." Are such color-emotion associations fundamental to our shared cognitive architecture, or are they cultural creations learned through our languages and traditions? To answer these questions, we tested emotional associations of colors in 4,598 participants from 30 nations speaking 22 native languages. Participants associated 20 emotion concepts with 12 color terms. Pattern-similarity analyses revealed universal color-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient r = .88). However, local differences were also apparent. A machine-learning algorithm revealed that nation predicted color-emotion associations above and beyond those observed universally. Similarity was greater when nations were linguistically or geographically close. This study highlights robust universal color-emotion associations, further modulated by linguistic and geographic factors. These results pose further theoretical and empirical questions about the affective properties of color and may inform practice in applied domains, such as well-being and design.


Assuntos
Emoções , Idioma , Cor , Percepção de Cores , Humanos , Ciúme , Linguística , Aprendizado de Máquina
10.
Hear Res ; 395: 108028, 2020 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32693189

RESUMO

Human loudness judgments of time-varying sounds show a non-uniform temporal weighting pattern with increased weights at the beginning of a sound. Four experiments were conducted to investigate whether this primacy effect reoccurs after a silent gap of an appropriate duration that is inserted into a level-fluctuating sound. In three of the experiments, contiguous sounds as well as sounds containing silent gaps of different durations were presented. The temporal loudness weights were compared between the sounds that contained a gap and the sounds without a gap. The data showed that with increasing gap duration an increasingly pronounced primacy effect reoccurred on the second sound part in the sense that a) the weights assigned to the first segments after the gap were increased compared to the conditions without a gap, and that b) the following weights again showed a decrease over time. This effect was statistically significant for gap durations of 350 ms and above. To investigate whether an attenuation in level can lead to the same results as a silent gap, segments in the middle part of a sound were attenuated in the fourth experiment, and the resulting weights were compared to conditions in which the middle segments were unattenuated or where a 700 ms silent gap was presented instead of the middle segments. An attenuation of 15 dB resulted in a significant reoccurrence of the primacy effect, although the effect was more pronounced for an attenuation of 30 dB and the silent gap. The results are discussed in the light of auditory nerve responses, masking effects on intensity resolution, and assumptions based on evidence integration processes.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Som , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Percepção Sonora
11.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 73(1): 29-54, 2020 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31476965

RESUMO

The texture of an object's surface influences its perceived spatial extent. For example, Hermann von Helmholtz reported that a square patch with black and white stripes appears elongated perpendicular to the stripes' orientation. This time-honoured finding stands in contrast with more recent recommendations by interior-design experts who suggest that stripe wall patterns make rooms appear elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. In a series of four experiments, we presented stripe wall patterns and varied the orientation of the stripes (horizontal vs. vertical) and their density (number of stripes per degree of visual angle). Subjects estimated the width and height of stereoscopically presented interior spaces. Stripe patterns with higher densities made rooms appear both wider and higher than did stripe patterns with lower densities or plain walls. In contrast to both the predictions from the Helmholtz-square and the design guidelines, this effect was only weakly modulated by pattern orientation, in the sense that rooms appeared elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. We conclude that object-based texture effects cannot be generalised to interior space perception. For a room's perceived spatial extent, pattern density is more important than pattern orientation.


Assuntos
Decoração de Interiores e Mobiliário , Percepção Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Orientação Espacial , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
12.
PLoS One ; 14(11): e0223075, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31689327

RESUMO

Previous research has consistently shown that for sounds varying in intensity over time, the beginning of the sound is of higher importance for the perception of loudness than later parts (primacy effect). However, in all previous studies, the target sounds were presented in quiet, and at a fixed average sound level. In the present study, temporal loudness weights for a time-varying narrowband noise were investigated in the presence of a continuous bandpass-filtered background noise and the average sound levels of the target stimuli were varied across a range of 60 dB. Pronounced primacy effects were observed in all conditions and there were no significant differences between the temporal weights observed in the conditions in quiet and in background noise. Within the conditions in background noise, there was a significant effect of the sound level on the pattern of weights, which was mainly caused by a slight trend for increased weights at the end of the sounds ("recency effect") in the condition with lower average level. No such effect was observed for the in-quiet conditions. Taken together, the observed primacy effect is largely independent of masking as well as of sound level. Compatible with this conclusion, the observed primacy effects in quiet and in background noise can be well described by an exponential decay function using parameters based on previous studies. Simulations using a model for the partial loudness of time-varying sounds in background noise showed that the model does not predict the observed temporal loudness weights.


Assuntos
Percepção Sonora/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Ruído , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Psicoacústica , Som , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
13.
R Soc Open Sci ; 6(9): 190741, 2019 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31598303

RESUMO

The link between colour and emotion and its possible similarity across cultures are questions that have not been fully resolved. Online, 711 participants from China, Germany, Greece and the UK associated 12 colour terms with 20 discrete emotion terms in their native languages. We propose a machine learning approach to quantify (a) the consistency and specificity of colour-emotion associations and (b) the degree to which they are country-specific, on the basis of the accuracy of a statistical classifier in (a) decoding the colour term evaluated on a given trial from the 20 ratings of colour-emotion associations and (b) predicting the country of origin from the 240 individual colour-emotion associations, respectively. The classifier accuracies were significantly above chance level, demonstrating that emotion associations are to some extent colour-specific and that colour-emotion associations are to some extent country-specific. A second measure of country-specificity, the in-group advantage of the colour-decoding accuracy, was detectable but relatively small (6.1%), indicating that colour-emotion associations are both universal and culture-specific. Our results show that machine learning is a promising tool when analysing complex datasets from emotion research.

14.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 144(2): EL119, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30180681

RESUMO

The onset of a sound receives a higher weight than later portions in time when its loudness is assessed, an effect commonly referred to as primacy effect. It is investigated if this effect can be predicted on the basis of an exponentially decaying function where the weight assigned to a temporal portion of a sound is the integral of this function over the segment duration. To test this model, temporal loudness weights were measured for sounds with different segment durations and total durations. The model successfully predicted essential aspects of the data.

15.
PLoS One ; 13(9): e0201976, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30183714

RESUMO

Previous studies have reported that bright ceilings appear higher than dark ceilings, irrespective of the other colorimetric properties of the ceiling color (hue, saturation) and irrespective of the luminance of the remaining room surfaces (walls, floor). In the present study, we expand these findings to width and depth estimates. We presented stereoscopic full-scale room simulations on a head-mounted display and varied the luminance of the side walls, rear wall, and ceiling independently of each other. Participants judged the width and depth of the simulated rooms. Our results show that the perceived spatial layout of a given room is significantly influenced by the luminance of the direct bounding surfaces (e.g., the side walls when judging perceived width) but less affected by the luminance of the other surfaces. In the discussion, we provide an overall picture of effects of surface luminance on the perceived layout of interior spaces and discuss the conclusions in the context of existing interior-design guidelines.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Cor , Decoração de Interiores e Mobiliário/métodos , Pintura , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Iluminação , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
16.
Hum Factors ; 60(8): 1228-1240, 2018 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30067403

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We investigate effects of the hue, saturation, and luminance of ceiling color on the perceived height of interior spaces. BACKGROUND: Previous studies have reported that the perceived height of an interior space is influenced by the luminance of the ceiling, but not by the luminance contrast between ceiling and walls: brighter ceilings appeared higher than darker ceilings, irrespective of wall and floor luminance. However, these studies used solely achromatic colors. We report an experiment in which we extend these findings to effects of chromatic ceiling colors. METHODS: We presented stereoscopic room simulations on a head-mounted display (Oculus Rift DK2) and varied hue (red, green, blue), saturation (low, high), and luminance (bright, dark) of the ceiling independently of each other. RESULTS: We found the previously reported ceiling luminance effect to apply also to chromatic colors: subjects judged brighter ceilings to be higher than darker ceilings. The remaining color dimensions merely had a very small (hue) or virtually no effect (saturation) on perceived height. CONCLUSION: In order to maximize the perceived height of an interior space, we suggest painting the ceiling in the brightest possible color. The hue and saturation of the paint are only of minor importance. APPLICATION: The present study improves the empirical basis for interior design guidelines regarding effects of surface color on the perceived layout of interior spaces.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos
17.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 143(2): 943, 2018 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29495718

RESUMO

Loudness is a fundamental aspect of auditory perception that is closely related to the physical level of the sound. However, it has been demonstrated that, in contrast to a sound level meter, human listeners do not weight all temporal segments of a sound equally. Instead, the beginning of a sound is more important for loudness estimation than later temporal portions. The present study investigates the mechanism underlying this primacy effect by varying the number of equal-duration temporal segments (5 and 20) and the total duration of the sound (1.0 to 10.0 s) in a factorial design. Pronounced primacy effects were observed for all 20-segment sounds. The temporal weights for the five-segment sounds are similar to those for the 20-segment sounds when the weights of the segments covering the same temporal range as a segment of the five-segment sounds are averaged. The primacy effect can be described by an exponential decay function with a time constant of about 200 ms. Thus, the temporal weight assigned to a specific temporal portion of a sound is determined by the time delay between sound onset and segment onset rather than by the number of segments or the total duration of the sound.

18.
Psychol Res ; 82(5): 896-914, 2018 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28612080

RESUMO

Previous studies on emotional effects of color often failed to control all the three perceptual dimensions of color: hue, saturation, and brightness. Here, we presented a three-dimensional space of chromatic colors by independently varying hue (blue, green, red), saturation (low, medium, high), and brightness (dark, medium, bright) in a factorial design. The 27 chromatic colors, plus 3 brightness-matched achromatic colors, were presented via an LED display. Participants (N = 62) viewed each color for 30 s and then rated their current emotional state (valence and arousal). Skin conductance and heart rate were measured continuously. The emotion ratings showed that saturated and bright colors were associated with higher arousal. The hue also had a significant effect on arousal, which increased from blue and green to red. The ratings of valence were the highest for saturated and bright colors, and also depended on the hue. Several interaction effects of the three color dimensions were observed for both arousal and valence. For instance, the valence ratings were higher for blue than for the remaining hues, but only for highly saturated colors. Saturated and bright colors caused significantly stronger skin conductance responses. Achromatic colors resulted in a short-term deceleration in the heart rate, while chromatic colors caused an acceleration. The results confirm that color stimuli have effects on the emotional state of the observer. These effects are not only determined by the hue of a color, as is often assumed, but by all the three color dimensions as well as their interactions.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Adulto , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Feminino , Resposta Galvânica da Pele/fisiologia , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
19.
PLoS One ; 12(4): e0175305, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28380077

RESUMO

This study investigated the role of vection (i.e., a visually induced sense of self-motion), optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), and inadvertent head movements in visually induced motion sickness (VIMS), evoked by yaw rotation of the visual surround. These three elements have all been proposed as contributing factors in VIMS, as they can be linked to different motion sickness theories. However, a full understanding of the role of each factor is still lacking because independent manipulation has proven difficult in the past. We adopted an integrative approach to the problem by obtaining measures of potentially relevant parameters in four experimental conditions and subsequently combining them in a linear mixed regression model. To that end, participants were exposed to visual yaw rotation in four separate sessions. Using a full factorial design, the OKN was manipulated by a fixation target (present/absent), and vection strength by introducing a conflict in the motion direction of the central and peripheral field of view (present/absent). In all conditions, head movements were minimized as much as possible. Measured parameters included vection strength, vection variability, OKN slow phase velocity, OKN frequency, the number of inadvertent head movements, and inadvertent head tilt. Results show that VIMS increases with vection strength, but that this relation varies among participants (R2 = 0.48). Regression parameters for vection variability, head and eye movement parameters were not significant. These results may seem to be in line with the Sensory Conflict theory on motion sickness, but we argue that a more detailed definition of the exact nature of the conflict is required to fully appreciate the relationship between vection and VIMS.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Enjoo devido ao Movimento/etiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Biológicos , Movimento (Física) , Enjoo devido ao Movimento/fisiopatologia , Nistagmo Optocinético/fisiologia , Rotação/efeitos adversos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
20.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 54: 44-64, 2017 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28391027

RESUMO

Numerous studies have reported that time perception and temporal processing are impaired in schizophrenia. In a meta-analytical review, we differentiate between time perception (judgments of time intervals) and basic temporal processing (e.g., judgments of temporal order) as well as between effects on accuracy (deviation of estimates from the veridical value) and precision (variability of judgments). In a meta-regression approach, we also included the specific tasks and the different time interval ranges as covariates. We considered 68 publications of the past 65years, and meta-analyzed data from 957 patients with schizophrenia and 1060 healthy control participants. Independent of tasks and interval durations, our results demonstrate that time perception and basic temporal processing are less precise (more variable) in patients (Hedges' g>1.00), whereas effects of schizophrenia on accuracy of time perception are rather small and task-dependent. Our review also shows that several aspects, e.g., potential influences of medication, have not yet been investigated in sufficient detail. In conclusion, the results are in accordance with theoretical assumptions and the notion of a more variable internal clock in patients with schizophrenia, but not with a strong effect of schizophrenia on clock speed. The impairment of temporal precision, however, may also be clock-unspecific as part of a general cognitive deficiency in schizophrenia.


Assuntos
Julgamento/fisiologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...