Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
1.
J Correct Health Care ; 25(3): 253-264, 2019 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31179818

RESUMO

This study examined the associations between arrest and incarceration, trait aggression, and emergency department (ED) use. Data were collected from 525 clients who visited the Center for Behavioral Research and Services in Long Beach, CA, using the following instruments: Risk Behavior Assessment, Risk Behavior Follow-Up Assessment, the Aggression Questionnaire (AQ), the Displaced AQ, and the parole and Legal Status section of the Addiction Severity Index. The bivariate analysis suggested that ED use was significantly associated with trait aggression and trait-displaced aggression among those with a history of incarceration. In the logistic regression, weapons offenses, manslaughter/homicide, being male, and being Black were significantly associated with ED use. Identification and management of aggressive trait personality are important in improving the management of postrelease care in transition to clinical networks and community-based health care settings.


Assuntos
Agressão , Criminosos/estatística & dados numéricos , Serviço Hospitalar de Emergência/estatística & dados numéricos , Negro ou Afro-Americano , Feminino , Homicídio/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Índices de Gravidade do Trauma , Armas/estatística & dados numéricos
2.
Int J STD AIDS ; 30(7): 647-655, 2019 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30961464

RESUMO

This study examined the comparative health risk behaviors of women who (a) traded sex for money, (b) traded sex for drugs, (c) traded sex for both drugs and money, or (d) did not trade sex. Self-report data were collected from 2369 women who received services through HIV and sexually transmitted infection (STI) testing programs and a subset were tested for HIV, hepatitis B, hepatitis C, and syphilis. Results revealed those women who traded sex only for money used condoms, were tested for HIV, and received the HIV test results more often than the other women. Women who traded sex for both drugs and money reported a significantly higher prevalence of gonorrhea, hepatitis B, and syphilis; were more likely to test positive for hepatitis B, syphilis, and HIV; engaged more often in sex acts without condoms; and were incarcerated for significantly more days. Based on these findings, the targets with greatest potential for STI prevention interventions are female sex workers who trade sex for both drugs and money.


Assuntos
Sexo Seguro/estatística & dados numéricos , Trabalho Sexual , Profissionais do Sexo/estatística & dados numéricos , Comportamento Sexual/estatística & dados numéricos , Abuso de Substâncias por Via Intravenosa/epidemiologia , Sexo sem Proteção/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , California/epidemiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Preservativos , Feminino , Gonorreia/epidemiologia , Infecções por HIV/epidemiologia , Hepatite B/epidemiologia , Humanos , Prevalência , Assunção de Riscos , Profissionais do Sexo/psicologia , Abuso de Substâncias por Via Intravenosa/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
3.
J Nerv Ment Dis ; 205(5): 372-379, 2017 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28230564

RESUMO

This study compares adults with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on measures of direct and displaced aggression and illicit drug use. Three hundred ninety-six adults were administered the Wender Utah Rating Scale, the Risk Behavior Assessment, the Aggression Questionnaire (AQ), and the Displaced Aggression Questionnaire (DAQ). Those with ADHD were higher on all scales of the AQ and DAQ, were younger at first use of amphetamines, and were more likely to have ever used crack and amphetamines. A Structural Equation Model found a significant interaction in that for those with medium and high levels of verbal aggression, ADHD predicts crack and amphetamine. Follow-up logistic regression models suggest that blacks self-medicate with crack and whites and Hispanics self-medicate with amphetamine when they have ADHD and verbal aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Anfetaminas/uso terapêutico , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/fisiopatologia , Cocaína Crack/uso terapêutico , Automedicação , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/etiologia , Adulto , Agressão/efeitos dos fármacos , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/tratamento farmacológico , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/etnologia , População Negra/etnologia , Feminino , Hispânico ou Latino/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Los Angeles/etnologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/etnologia , População Branca/etnologia
4.
Addict Behav ; 60: 24-31, 2016 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27082265

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: This study compared women who sex trade for drugs, money, or both compared to neither (did not sex trade), and introduced the concept of trait displaced aggression to the literature on sex trading. METHODS: Female participants (n=1055) were recruited from a low-income area of southern California. Measures included: the Risk Behavior Assessment (RBA), Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS), Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale (EIS), and the Displaced Aggression Questionnaire (DAQ). RESULTS: Women who traded sex for both drugs and money used crack cocaine, powder cocaine, and alcohol significantly more, scored higher on the BIS, and the EIS, and were significantly older. Those who only sex traded for drugs used more amphetamine, heroin, and injected drugs more days. They were also higher on the DAQ and all of the DAQ subscales. Those who traded for money only used marijuana more and were more likely to use marijuana before sex. CONCLUSIONS: This study may help address specific issues unique to those who sex trade for different commodities in that the drugs used are different and the underlying personality characteristics are different.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Comportamento Impulsivo , Trabalho Sexual/psicologia , Trabalho Sexual/estatística & dados numéricos , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/epidemiologia , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/psicologia , Adolescente , California , Feminino , Humanos
5.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(8): 1037-1049, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24854477

RESUMO

Considerable research has shown that alcohol consumption can increase aggression and produce extremes in other social behaviors. Although most theories posit that such effects are caused by pharmacological impairment of cognitive processes, recent research indicates that exposure to alcohol-related constructs, in the absence of consumption, can produce similar effects. Here we tested the hypothesis that alcohol priming is most likely to affect aggression in the context of ambiguous provocation. Experiment 1 showed that exposure to alcohol primes increased aggressive retaliation but only when an initial provocation was ambiguous; unambiguous provocation elicited highly aggressive responses regardless of prime exposure. Experiment 2 showed that alcohol prime exposure effects are relatively short-lived and that perceptions of the provocateur's hostility mediated effects of prime exposure on aggression. These findings suggest modification and extension of existing models of alcohol-induced aggression.

6.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(1): 79-91, 2014 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24072753

RESUMO

The trait and social cognitive perspectives are considered disparate approaches to understanding personality. We suggest an integrative view in which three elements derived from the social cognitive perspective (i.e., situations, behaviors, and explanations [SBEs]) form the basis of personality traits. Study 1 demonstrated strong associations between traits and SBEs across the Big Five dimensions. Studies 2 through 7 tested the discriminative validity, internal structure, and unique contributions of the individual components of SBEs. Studies 8 and 9 demonstrated that the strong associations between traits and SBEs generalize to different cultures. The present work suggests that SBEs may be a universal folk psychological mechanism underlying personality traits.


Assuntos
Personalidade , Autoimagem , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Int J Psychol Res (Medellin) ; 6: 30-41, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26023325

RESUMO

In social neuroscience research, startle eyeblink modification can serve as a marker of emotion, but it is less clear whether it can also serve as a marker of prejudice. In Experiment 1, 30 White students viewed photographs of White and Black targets while the startle eyeblink reflex and facial EMG from the brow and cheek regions were recorded. Prejudice was related to facial EMG activity, but not to startle modification, which instead appeared to index attention to race. To test further whether racial categorizations are associated with differential attention, a dual-task paradigm was used in Experiment 2. Fifty-four White and fifty-five Black participants responded more slowly to a tone presented when viewing a racial outgroup member or a negative stimulus, indicating that both draw more attention than ingroup members or positive stimuli. We conclude that startle modification is useful to index differential attention to groups when intergroup threat is low.

9.
Aggress Behav ; 39(1): 13-29, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23042637

RESUMO

Four studies present the first evidence showing that public (vs. private) provocation augments triggered displaced aggression by increasing the perceived intensity of the provocation. This effect is shown to be independent of face-saving motivation. Following a public or private provocation, Study 1 participants were induced to ruminate or were distracted for 20 min. They then had an opportunity to aggress against another person who either acted in a neutral or mildly annoying fashion (viz. triggering event). As expected, the magnitude of the greater displaced aggression of those who ruminated before the triggering event compared with those distracted was greater under public than private provocation. Study 2 replicated the findings of Study 1 and confirmed that public provocations are experienced as more intense. Studies 3 and 4 both manipulated provocation intensity directly to show that it mediated the moderating effect of public/private provocation found in Study 1. The greater intensity of a public provocation increases reactivity to a subsequent trigger, which in turn, augments triggered displaced aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Ira , Motivação , Pensamento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo
10.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 50(Pt 2): 281-301, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21545459

RESUMO

Although rumination following a provocation can increase aggression, no research has examined the processes responsible for this phenomenon. With predictions derived from the General Aggression Model, three experiments explored the impact of two types of post-provocation rumination on the processes whereby rumination augments aggression. In Experiment 1, relative to distraction, self-focused rumination uniquely increased the accessibility of arousal cognition, whereas provocation-focused rumination uniquely amplified the accessibility of aggressive action cognition. In Experiment 2, provocation-focused rumination uniquely increased systolic blood pressure. In Experiment 3, both types of rumination increased aggressive behaviour relative to a distraction condition. Angry affect partially mediated the effects of both provocation- and self-focused rumination on aggression. Self-critical negative affect partially mediated the effect of self-focused rumination but not provocation-focused rumination. These findings suggest that provocation-focused rumination influences angry affect, aggressive action cognition, and cardiovascular arousal, whereas self-focused rumination increases self-critical negative affect, angry affect, and arousal cognition. These studies enhance our understanding of why two types of post-provocation rumination increase aggressive behaviour.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Emoções , Pensamento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos
11.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 37(6): 850-62, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21421767

RESUMO

Interpersonal provocation is a common and robust antecedent to aggression. Four studies identified angry rumination and reduced self-control as mechanisms underlying the provocation-aggression relationship. Following provocation, participants demonstrated decreased self-control on an unpleasant task relative to a control condition (Study 1). When provoked, rumination reduced self-control and increased aggression. This effect was mediated by reduced self-control capacity (Study 2). State rumination following provocation, but not anger per se, mediated the effect of trait rumination on aggression (Study 3). Bolstering self-regulatory resources by consuming a glucose beverage improved performance on a measure of inhibitory control following rumination (Study 4). These findings suggest that rumination following an anger-inducing provocation reduces self-control and increases aggression. Bolstering self-regulatory resources may reduce this adverse effect.


Assuntos
Agressão , Ira , Cognição , Comportamento Impulsivo , Controle Interno-Externo , Relações Interpessoais , Adolescente , California , Feminino , Glucose/administração & dosagem , Humanos , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
12.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 21(4): 734-44, 2009 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18578600

RESUMO

Very little is known about the neural circuitry guiding anger, angry rumination, and aggressive personality. In the present fMRI experiment, participants were insulted and induced to ruminate. Activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex was positively related to self-reported feelings of anger and individual differences in general aggression. Activity in the medial prefrontal cortex was related to self-reported rumination and individual differences in displaced aggression. Increased activation in the hippocampus, insula, and cingulate cortex following the provocation predicted subsequent self-reported rumination. These findings increase our understanding of the neural processes associated with the risk for aggressive behavior by specifying neural regions that mediate the subjective experience of anger and angry rumination as well as the neural pathways linked to different types of aggressive behavior.


Assuntos
Sintomas Afetivos/patologia , Agressão , Ira , Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Sintomas Afetivos/fisiopatologia , Agressão/psicologia , Encéfalo/irrigação sanguínea , Encéfalo/patologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Imageamento Tridimensional/métodos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Oxigênio/sangue , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
13.
Open Addict J ; 2: 39-47, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21165162

RESUMO

Drug use is considered a main contributing factor to crime and violence. This research examined the evidence regarding the relationship between drug abuse and the occurrence of intimate partner violence. Current drug using men were assessed on aggression related personality variables, their drug use, and the occurrence of violence in their close relationships. A latent aggression factor and recent amphetamine use were the only variables found to be significantly associated with violence. No other drug use variables were found to be associated with violence by the participant and the overall drug use factor was not found to be associated with violence or aggressive personality. The widely accepted notion that increased substance use directly leads to increases in violent behavior was only partially supported, at least within this drug using population. The assessment of aggressive personality, rather than of drug use, is suggested for correctional as well as clinical settings in which drug users are prevalent when determining susceptibility to violence.

14.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 34(10): 1382-95, 2008 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18660383

RESUMO

Sometimes aggression is displaced onto a target who is not totally innocent but emits a mildly irritating behavior called a triggering event. In three experiments, the authors examine stable personal attributes of targets that can impact such triggered displaced aggression (TDA). Lower levels of TDA were directed to targets whose attitudes were similar as compared to dissimilar to those of the actor (Experiment 1) and to targets who were ingroup as compared to out-group members (Experiment 2). Conceptually replicating the findings of Experiments 1 and 2, the manipulated valence of the target (viz., liked, neutral, and disliked) functioned in a similar manner, with positive valence serving a buffering function against a triggering action that followed an initial provocation (Experiment 3). The results from all three experiments are consistent with cognitive neoassociationist theory.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Deslocamento Psicológico , Processos Grupais , Comportamento Social , Desejabilidade Social , Percepção Social , Atitude , Cognição , Vítimas de Crime , Feminino , Hostilidade , Humanos , Masculino , Grupo Associado
15.
Aggress Behav ; 33(6): 477-85, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17595007

RESUMO

We tested the hypothesis that ruminating about a previous aggressive prime interacts with a subsequent minor frustration to augment aggression. Sixty participants watched a video showing a murder during a bank robbery (the aggressive prime). Those in the rumination condition were asked to write about the video for 20 min. In the no rumination condition, participants were given 20 min to complete an irrelevant task. Participants were then either frustrated or not frustrated. Our results supported the main hypothesis. Relative to the control condition, neither rumination nor frustration alone impacted aggression. Rumination, in combination with a minor frustration, however, increased the recommended prison sentence towards the targets. We discuss the implications of our findings.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Atitude , Frustração , Rememoração Mental , Prisões/legislação & jurisprudência , Adulto , California , Crime/legislação & jurisprudência , Crime/psicologia , Feminino , Homicídio , Humanos , Jurisprudência , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários , Gravação de Videoteipe , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
16.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 90(6): 1032-51, 2006 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16784350

RESUMO

Previous measures of aggressive personality have focused on direct aggression (i.e., retaliation toward the provoking agent). An original self-report measure of trait displaced aggression is presented. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses provided support for a 3-factor conceptualization of the construct. These analyses identified an affective dimension (angry rumination), a cognitive dimension (revenge planning), and a behavioral dimension (general tendency to engage in displaced aggression). The trait measure demonstrated good internal consistency and test-retest reliability as well as convergent and discriminant construct validity. Unlike other related personality measures, trait displaced aggression significantly predicted indirect indicators of real-world displaced aggression (i.e., self-reported domestic abuse and road rage) as well as laboratory displaced aggression in 2 experiments.


Assuntos
Agressão , Deslocamento Psicológico , Determinação da Personalidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , California , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Inquéritos e Questionários
17.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 88(6): 969-83, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15982116

RESUMO

Ruminating about a provocation increases the likelihood of displaced aggression following a minor annoyance (trigger). In Study 1, provoked participants who ruminated for 25 min were more aggressive toward a fumbling confederate than were distracted participants. Provocation-induced negative affect was positively related to aggression but only among those who ruminated. Study 2 conceptually replicated Study 1 and also found that the more negatively people reacted to the trigger, the more likely the trigger was to increase displaced aggression. Study 3 replicated the findings of Studies 1 and 2 by using an 8-hr rumination period. All 3 studies suggest that ruminating about a provocation increases the likelihood that a minor triggering annoyance will increase displaced aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Comportamento Social , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
18.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 87(2): 125-54, 2004 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14757068

RESUMO

A word-learning task was used to investigate variation among developmental dyslexics classified as phonological and surface dyslexics. Dyslexic children and chronological age (CA)- and reading level (RL)-matched normal readers were taught to pronounce novel nonsense words such as veep. Words were assigned either a regular (e.g., "veep") or an irregular (e.g., "vip") pronunciation. Phonological dyslexics learned both regular and exception words more slowly than the normal readers and, unlike the other groups, did not show a regular-word advantage. Surface dyslexics also learned regular and exception words more slowly than the CA group, consistent with a specific problem in mastering arbitrary item-specific pronunciations, but their performance resembled that of the RL group. The results parallel earlier findings from Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride-Chang, & Petersen [Cognition 58 (1996) 157-195] indicating that surface dyslexics and phonological dyslexics have a different profile of reading deficits, with surface dyslexics resembling younger normal readers and phonological dyslexics showing a specific phonological deficit. Models of reading and reading disability need to account for the heterogeneity in reading processes among dyslexic children.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Articulação/fisiopatologia , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Dislexia/psicologia , Aprendizagem , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Percepção Visual
19.
Pers Soc Psychol Rev ; 7(1): 57-97, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12584058

RESUMO

A tit-for-tat matching rule (Axelrod, 1984) describes much interpersonal behavior. Yet, in daily life a retaliatory aggressive response to a trivially mild provocation often inappropriately exceeds that expected from the matching rule. The concept of triggered displaced aggression can explain these exceptions to the matching principle. Building from the Cognitive Neoassociationistic model of aggressive behavior (Berkowitz, 1989, 1990, 1993), we developed a theoretical framework of social and personality factors that moderate and mediate the disjunctively escalated retaliation that can result from triggered displaced aggression. Major explanatory factors in our analysis of such effects are as follows: (a) aspects of the Time 1 provocation and the immediate situation in which it occurred; (b) characteristics of initial provocations and personality factors of the actor that produce the ruminative thought that will temporally extend the effects of a Time 1 provocation, allowing them to interact with a delayed Time 2 minor triggering event; and (c) actions and attributes of the target of displaced aggression that augment these effects.


Assuntos
Agressão , Teoria Psicológica , Humanos
20.
Psychol Sci ; 13(2): 157-61, 2002 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11934000

RESUMO

Do men seek more short-term mates than women? Buss and Schmitt (1993) showed a pattern of mean difference in the ideal number of sexual partners men and women desired over various time frames. We replicated these mean sex differences (e.g., ideal number over the next 30 years: Ms = 7.69 and 2.78 for men and women, respectively), but in both data sets the sampling distributions were highly skewed. In Study 1, we found few sex differences in medians across time frames (e.g., ideal number over the next 30 years: Mdn = 1 for both men and women). In Study 2, most college men (98.9%) and women (99.2%) said they wanted to settle down with one mutually exclusive sexual partner at some point in their life, ideally within the next 5 years. Neither medians in number of partners desired overall before settling down (replicating Study 1) nor medians in short-term partners desired before settling down (Mdn = 0) differed significantly by gender. Rather, men and women concurred: Short-term mating is not what humans typically seek.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Identidade de Gênero , Comportamento Sexual , Parceiros Sexuais , Adulto , Comportamento de Escolha , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Casamento
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...