Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 18 de 18
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Behav Neurosci ; 135(1): 24-31, 2021 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33856842

RESUMO

Adult neurogenesis increases in mammals when they are exposed to an enriched environment or given the opportunity to exercise. In this experiment, we investigated whether turtles would show differences in the number of new neurons in the telencephalon when they were exposed to deep water, conspecifics, and plants and logs (EE group), compared to a group of animals housed in individual cages with shallow water (IN group). A control group (EX) was given deep water and conspecifics but no plants and logs. We gave nine injections of BrdU over a 3-week period, starting when the turtles were introduced to the housing. The results showed that both the EE and the EX groups had more new cells in the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR), a sensory area of the telencephalon. The two groups did not differ from one another. The group-housed animals also had a higher percentage of new neurons in the DVR that were double labeled for NeuN, a marker of neurons, compared to the IN group. There were no significant differences between groups in the number of new cells in the medial cortex, the homolog of the hippocampus. These findings demonstrate that the housing experience influences the number of new cells that survive in the brains of turtles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Encéfalo/citologia , Meio Ambiente , Abrigo para Animais , Neurogênese , Neurônios/citologia , Tartarugas/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Animais , Encéfalo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Feminino , Hipocampo/citologia , Masculino , Telencéfalo/citologia
2.
Brain Behav Evol ; 92(1-2): 71-81, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30176658

RESUMO

Research on mammals and turtles has suggested that acetylcholine is involved in attention in these groups. Two experiments investigated the ability of painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) to ignore irrelevant stimuli when the basal forebrain acetylcholine system was compromised. In experiment 1, turtles given lesions of the basal magnocellular cholinergic nucleus (NBM) or sham lesions were tested on a go/no go discrimination between horizontal and vertical stripes with or without irrelevant inserts in the box. The irrelevant inserts were blue and white checked walls and green carpet on the floor. The group with lesions of the NBM and no irrelevant inserts had no difficulty learning the task, but the lesioned group with irrelevant inserts was impaired on the discrimination. The sham-lesioned group was not impaired by the presence of irrelevant inserts. In experiment 2, turtles were given either the acetylcholine muscarinic receptor blocker scopolamine or saline and tested on the same task. The turtles given scopolamine had no difficulty learning the task in the absence of irrelevant inserts, but they were severely impaired when irrelevant inserts were present. The irrelevant inserts did not affect the learning of control turtles given saline. These findings provide evidence that acetylcholine enhances turtles' ability to orient to relevant stimuli and suggest that its role in learning and memory may be to allow animals to orient to the stimuli relevant to a task and to ignore irrelevant stimuli.


Assuntos
Acetilcolina/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Prosencéfalo Basal , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Antagonistas Muscarínicos/farmacologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tartarugas/fisiologia , Acetilcolina/metabolismo , Animais , Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Prosencéfalo Basal/efeitos dos fármacos , Prosencéfalo Basal/metabolismo , Prosencéfalo Basal/fisiopatologia , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Aprendizagem/efeitos dos fármacos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Escopolamina/farmacologia , Tartarugas/metabolismo
3.
Brain Behav Evol ; 87(3): 175-183, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27559734

RESUMO

Studies of the relationship between behavioral plasticity and new cells in the adult brain in amphibians and reptiles are sparse but demonstrate that environmental and hormonal variables do have an effect on the amount of cell proliferation and/or migration. The variables that are reviewed here are: enriched environment, social stimulation, spatial area use, season, photoperiod and temperature, and testosterone. Fewer data are available for amphibians than for reptiles, but for both groups many issues are still to be resolved. It is to be hoped that the questions raised here will generate more answers in future studies.

4.
Brain Behav Evol ; 87(3): 143-145, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27560929
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 109(3): 734-41, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23155174

RESUMO

Memory-guided saccades are slower than visually guided saccades. The usual explanation for this slowing is that the absence of a visual drive reduces the discharge of neurons in the superior colliculus. We tested a related hypothesis: that the slowing of memory-guided saccades was due also to the more frequent occurrence of gaze-evoked blinks with memory-guided saccades compared with visually guided saccades. We recorded gaze-evoked blinks in three monkeys while they performed visually guided and memory-guided saccades and compared the kinematics of the different saccade types with and without blinks. Gaze-evoked blinks were more common during memory-guided saccades than during visually guided saccades, and the well-established relationship between peak and average velocity for saccades was disrupted by blinking. The occurrence of gaze-evoked blinks was associated with a greater slowing of memory-guided saccades compared with visually guided saccades. Likewise, when blinks were absent, the peak velocity of visually guided saccades was only slightly higher than that of memory-guided saccades. Our results reveal interactions between circuits generating saccades and blink-evoked eye movements. The interaction leads to increased curvature of saccade trajectories and a corresponding decrease in saccade velocity. Consistent with this interpretation, the amount of saccade curvature and slowing increased with gaze-evoked blink amplitude. Thus, although the absence of vision decreases the velocity of memory-guided saccades relative to visually guided saccades somewhat, the cooccurrence of gaze-evoked blinks produces the majority of slowing for memory-guided saccades.


Assuntos
Piscadela/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Animais , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Macaca mulatta , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Colículos Superiores/citologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia
7.
J Neurosci ; 31(31): 11256-67, 2011 Aug 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21813686

RESUMO

Although spontaneous blinking is one of the most frequent human movements, little is known about its neural basis. We developed a rat model of spontaneous blinking to identify and better characterize the spontaneous blink generator. We monitored spontaneous blinking for 55 min periods in normal conditions and after the induction of mild dry eye or dopaminergic drug challenges. The normal spontaneous blink rate was 5.3 ± 0.3 blinks/min. Dry eye or 1 mg/kg apomorphine significantly increased and 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol significantly decreased the blink rate. Additional analyses revealed a consistent temporal organization to spontaneous blinking with a median 750 s period that was independent of the spontaneous blink rate. Dry eye and dopaminergic challenges significantly modified the regularity of the normal pattern of episodes of frequent blinking interspersed with intervals having few blinks. Dry eye and apomorphine enhanced the regularity of this pattern, whereas haloperidol reduced its regularity. The simplest explanation for our data is that the spinal trigeminal complex is a critical element in the generation of spontaneous blinks, incorporating reflex blinks from dry eye and indirect basal ganglia inputs into the blink generator. Although human subjects exhibited a higher average blink rate (17.6 ± 2.4) than rats, the temporal pattern of spontaneous blinking was qualitatively similar for both species. These data demonstrate that rats are an appropriate model for investigating the neural basis of human spontaneous blinking and suggest that the spinal trigeminal complex is a major element in the spontaneous blink generator.


Assuntos
Piscadela/fisiologia , Síndromes do Olho Seco/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Animais , Apomorfina/farmacologia , Piscadela/efeitos dos fármacos , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Agonistas de Dopamina/farmacologia , Antagonistas de Dopamina/farmacologia , Síndromes do Olho Seco/etiologia , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Análise de Fourier , Lateralidade Funcional , Haloperidol/farmacologia , Humanos , Magnetismo , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Ratos , Fatores de Tempo , Nervo Trigêmeo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
Brain Cogn ; 74(1): 10-7, 2010 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20579798

RESUMO

This study examined predictions based upon the right hemisphere (RH) model, the valence-arousal model, and a recently proposed integrated model (Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2007) of emotion processing by testing immediate recall and recognition memory for positive, negative, and neutral verbal stimuli among 35 right-handed women. Building upon methodologies of previous studies, we found that words presented to the right visual field/left hemisphere (RVF/LH) were recalled and recognized more accurately than words presented to the left visual field/right hemisphere (LVF/RH), and we found significant valence by visual field interactions. Some findings were consistent with one of the models evaluated whereas others were consistent with none of the models evaluated. Our findings suggest that an integration of the RH and valence-arousal models may best account for the findings with regard to hemispheric lateralization of memory for emotional stimuli.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adolescente , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 103(2): 641-7, 2010 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19939960

RESUMO

Based on kinematic properties and distinct substrates, there are different classes of eyelid movement described as eyeblinks. We investigate whether the eyelid movements made in response to a conditioned stimulus (CS) are a category of eyelid movements distinct from blinks. Human subjects received 60 trials of classical eyelid conditioning with a tone as the CS and electrical stimulation of the supraorbital branch of the trigeminal nerve as the unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Before and after training, reflex blinks were elicited with the UCS. The kinematics of conditioned responses (CRs) differed significantly from those of reflex blinks. The slope of the amplitude-maximum velocity function was steeper for reflex blinks than for CRs, and reflex blink duration was significantly shorter than CR duration. Unlike reflex blinks, for which maximum velocity was independent of blink duration, the maximum velocity of CRs depended on CR duration. These quantitative and qualitative differences indicated that CRs were a unique class of eyelid movements distinct from blinks and eyelid movements with vertical saccadic gaze shifts.


Assuntos
Piscadela/fisiologia , Condicionamento Palpebral/fisiologia , Pálpebras/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Behav Neurosci ; 123(4): 804-9, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19634938

RESUMO

Turtles were run on a negative patterning task involving 2 positive elements, a key with white stripes on a black background, and a solid red key, and a compound stimulus combining the 2 elements, white stripes on a red background. Injections of scopolamine, methylscopolamine, or saline were started at the same time that the compound stimulus was introduced, after the animals had been autoshaped to press the key for each of the elements. Scopolamine disrupted the learning of negative patterning, but methylscopolamine had no effect. In contrast, learning of a simple discrimination between the elements was not affected by scopolamine. These results show that muscarinic cholinergic receptors are involved in the learning of negative patterning in turtles.


Assuntos
Acetilcolina/metabolismo , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Tartarugas/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Antagonistas Colinérgicos/farmacologia , Aprendizagem/efeitos dos fármacos , Antagonistas Muscarínicos/farmacologia , N-Metilescopolamina/farmacologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Probabilidade , Distribuição Aleatória , Escopolamina/farmacologia
11.
Emotion ; 8(4): 552-9, 2008 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18729585

RESUMO

This study tested predictions based on the emotion context insensitivity (ECI) hypothesis of Rottenberg, Gross, and Gotlib (2005) that a nonclinical sample of people with depressive symptoms would show reduced responses to both positive and negative stimuli relative to people without depression and would show an enhanced response to novelty. Seventy individuals completed diagnostic questionnaires, made ratings of 21 affectively valenced pictures, and then viewed the same 21 pictures and 21 novel pictures while startle blink responses were recorded from electromyographic activity of the orbicularis oculi. People with scores on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) indicative of depression demonstrated a lack of affective startle modulation compared to the nondepression group. For all participants, the startle response was larger for novel pictures than for previously viewed pictures, but scores on the BDI were not related to response to novelty. Taken together, the results suggest that nonclinical depression is associated with a lack of affective modulation of startle, as has been shown for clinical depression.


Assuntos
Afeto , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Piscadela , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reflexo de Sobressalto , Inquéritos e Questionários , Percepção Visual
12.
Behav Res Ther ; 43(11): 1391-424, 2005 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15885654

RESUMO

Fear conditioning represents the process by which a neutral stimulus comes to evoke fear following its repeated pairing with an aversive stimulus. Although fear conditioning has long been considered a central pathogenic mechanism in anxiety disorders, studies employing lab-based conditioning paradigms provide inconsistent support for this idea. A quantitative review of 20 such studies, representing fear-learning scores for 453 anxiety patients and 455 healthy controls, was conducted to verify the aggregated result of this literature and to assess the moderating influences of study characteristics. Results point to modest increases in both acquisition of fear learning and conditioned responding during extinction among anxiety patients. Importantly, these patient-control differences are not apparent when looking at discrimination studies alone and primarily emerge from studies employing simple, single-cue paradigms where only danger cues are presented and no inhibition of fear to safety cues is required.


Assuntos
Transtornos de Ansiedade/psicologia , Condicionamento Clássico , Sinais (Psicologia) , Medo , Aprendizagem por Associação , Extinção Psicológica , Habituação Psicofisiológica , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Reforço Psicológico
13.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 6(2): 151-9, 2005 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15685220

RESUMO

We believe that names have a powerful influence on the experiments we do and the way in which we think. For this reason, and in the light of new evidence about the function and evolution of the vertebrate brain, an international consortium of neuroscientists has reconsidered the traditional, 100-year-old terminology that is used to describe the avian cerebrum. Our current understanding of the avian brain - in particular the neocortex-like cognitive functions of the avian pallium - requires a new terminology that better reflects these functions and the homologies between avian and mammalian brains.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Animais , Aves , Humanos , Vertebrados
14.
Behav Neurosci ; 119(6): 1656-61, 2005 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16420168

RESUMO

Turtles (Chrysemys picta) were given the nitric oxide synthase inhibitor NW-nitro-L-arginine methyl ester (L-NAME) or its inactive isomer NW-nitro-D-arginine methyl ester (D-NAME) and were trained on a negative patterning task or a simple go/no-go discrimination task. L-NAME impaired the learning of negative patterning but did not affect retention of the task if it had already been learned. D-NAME had no effect. Go/no-go discrimination learning was not affected by L-NAME. These findings support the notion that nitric oxide plays a role in complex configural learning in a reptile closely related to the ancestors of mammals.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , NG-Nitroarginina Metil Éster/farmacologia , Óxido Nítrico/antagonistas & inibidores , Tartarugas/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Comportamento de Escolha/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Inibidores Enzimáticos/farmacologia , Aprendizagem/efeitos dos fármacos , NG-Nitroarginina Metil Éster/análogos & derivados , Probabilidade , Fatores de Tempo
15.
J Comp Neurol ; 473(3): 377-414, 2004 May 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15116397

RESUMO

The standard nomenclature that has been used for many telencephalic and related brainstem structures in birds is based on flawed assumptions of homology to mammals. In particular, the outdated terminology implies that most of the avian telencephalon is a hypertrophied basal ganglia, when it is now clear that most of the avian telencephalon is neurochemically, hodologically, and functionally comparable to the mammalian neocortex, claustrum, and pallial amygdala (all of which derive from the pallial sector of the developing telencephalon). Recognizing that this promotes misunderstanding of the functional organization of avian brains and their evolutionary relationship to mammalian brains, avian brain specialists began discussions to rectify this problem, culminating in the Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum held at Duke University in July 2002, which approved a new terminology for avian telencephalon and some allied brainstem cell groups. Details of this new terminology are presented here, as is a rationale for each name change and evidence for any homologies implied by the new names. Revisions for the brainstem focused on vocal control, catecholaminergic, cholinergic, and basal ganglia-related nuclei. For example, the Forum recognized that the hypoglossal nucleus had been incorrectly identified as the nucleus intermedius in the Karten and Hodos (1967) pigeon brain atlas, and what was identified as the hypoglossal nucleus in that atlas should instead be called the supraspinal nucleus. The locus ceruleus of this and other avian atlases was noted to consist of a caudal noradrenergic part homologous to the mammalian locus coeruleus and a rostral region corresponding to the mammalian A8 dopaminergic cell group. The midbrain dopaminergic cell group in birds known as the nucleus tegmenti pedunculopontinus pars compacta was recognized as homologous to the mammalian substantia nigra pars compacta and was renamed accordingly; a group of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic neurons at the lateral edge of this region was identified as homologous to the mammalian substantia nigra pars reticulata and was also renamed accordingly. A field of cholinergic neurons in the rostral avian hindbrain was named the nucleus pedunculopontinus tegmenti, whereas the anterior nucleus of the ansa lenticularis in the avian diencephalon was renamed the subthalamic nucleus, both for their evident mammalian homologues. For the basal (i.e., subpallial) telencephalon, the actual parts of the basal ganglia were given names reflecting their now evident homologues. For example, the lobus parolfactorius and paleostriatum augmentatum were acknowledged to make up the dorsal subdivision of the striatal part of the basal ganglia and were renamed as the medial and lateral striatum. The paleostriatum primitivum was recognized as homologous to the mammalian globus pallidus and renamed as such. Additionally, the rostroventral part of what was called the lobus parolfactorius was acknowledged as comparable to the mammalian nucleus accumbens, which, together with the olfactory tubercle, was noted to be part of the ventral striatum in birds. A ventral pallidum, a basal cholinergic cell group, and medial and lateral bed nuclei of the stria terminalis were also recognized. The dorsal (i.e., pallial) telencephalic regions that had been erroneously named to reflect presumed homology to striatal parts of mammalian basal ganglia were renamed as part of the pallium, using prefixes that retain most established abbreviations, to maintain continuity with the outdated nomenclature. We concluded, however, that one-to-one (i.e., discrete) homologies with mammals are still uncertain for most of the telencephalic pallium in birds and thus the new pallial terminology is largely devoid of assumptions of one-to-one homologies with mammals. The sectors of the hyperstriatum composing the Wulst (i.e., the hyperstriatum accessorium intermedium, and dorsale), the hyperstriatum ventrale, the neostriatum, and the archistriatum have been renamed (respectively) the hyperpallium (hypertrophied pallium), the mesopallium (middle pallium), the nidopallium (nest pallium), and the arcopallium (arched pallium). The posterior part of the archistriatum has been renamed the posterior pallial amygdala, the nucleus taeniae recognized as part of the avian amygdala, and a region inferior to the posterior paleostriatum primitivum included as a subpallial part of the avian amygdala. The names of some of the laminae and fiber tracts were also changed to reflect current understanding of the location of pallial and subpallial sectors of the avian telencephalon. Notably, the lamina medularis dorsalis has been renamed the pallial-subpallial lamina. We urge all to use this new terminology, because we believe it will promote better communication among neuroscientists. Further information is available at http://avianbrain.org


Assuntos
Aves/anatomia & histologia , Tronco Encefálico/anatomia & histologia , Telencéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Terminologia como Assunto , Animais , Técnicas Histológicas , Imuno-Histoquímica , Ratos
16.
J Comp Neurol ; 473: E1-E6, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19626136

RESUMO

Many of the assumptions of homology on which the standard nomenclature for the cell groups and fiber tracts of avian brains have been based are in error, and as a result that terminology promotes misunderstanding of the functional organization of avian brains and their evolutionary relationship to mammalian brains. Recognizing this problem, a number of avian brain researchers began an effort to revise the terminology, which culminated in the Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum, held at Duke University from July 18 to 20, 2002. In the new terminology approved at this Forum, the flawed conception that the telencephalon of birds consists nearly entirely of a hypertrophied basal ganglia has been purged from the telencephalic terminology, and the actual parts of the basal ganglia and its brainstem afferent cell groups have been given names reflecting their now evident homologies. The telencephalic regions that were erroneously named to reflect presumed homology to mammalian basal ganglia were renamed as parts of the pallium, using prefixes that retained most established abbreviations (to maintain continuity with the replaced nomenclature). Details of this meeting and its major conclusions are presented in this paper, and the details of the new terminology and its basis are presented in a longer companion paper. We urge all to use this new terminology, because we believe it will promote better communication among neuroscientists.

17.
Biol Psychol ; 63(2): 179-97, 2003 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12738407

RESUMO

The potential moderating effect of sensation seeking on anxious reactivity to threatening experiences was assessed using the affective modulation of startle-blink paradigm. Startle blinks, as measured by electromyographic (EMG) activity in response to loud (100 dB) white-noise stimuli, were elicited during the presentation of positive, neutral, and threatening visual images. Unlike participants low in sensation seeking who showed blink potentiation during threatening versus neutral images, participants high in sensation seeking showed equal magnitudes of startle to neutral and threatening images. The results suggest that individuals high compared with low on sensation seeking are less anxiously reactive to physically threatening visual stimuli. No attenuation in startle magnitude was elicited by positive images among low or high sensation seekers suggesting that the positive images employed in the current study were not arousing enough to activate the appetitive arousal system.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Piscadela/fisiologia , Comportamento Exploratório , Medo , Reflexo de Sobressalto/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Sensação , Percepção Visual
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...