Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
1.
Soc Sci Med ; 348: 116787, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38547807

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Using a large longitudinal sample of adults from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study, the present study extended a recently developed hierarchical model to determine how best to model the accumulation of stressors, and to determine whether the rate of change in stressors or traditional composite scores of stressors are stronger predictors of health outcomes. METHOD: We used factor analysis to estimate a stress-factor score and then, to operationalize the accumulation of stressors we examined five approaches to aggregating information about repeated exposures to multiple stressors. The predictive validity of these approaches was then assessed in relation to different health outcomes. RESULTS: The prediction of chronic conditions, body mass index, difficulty with activities of daily living, executive function, and episodic memory later in life was strongest when the accumulation of stressors was modeled using total area under the curve (AUC) of estimated factor scores, compared to composite scores that have traditionally been used in studies of cumulative stress, as well as linear rates of change. CONCLUSIONS: Like endogenous, biological markers of stress reactivity, AUC for individual trajectories of self-reported stressors shows promise as a data reduction technique to model the accumulation of stressors in longitudinal studies. Overall, our results indicate that considering different quantitative models is critical to understanding the sequelae and predictive power of psychosocial stressors from midlife to late adulthood.


Assuntos
Estresse Psicológico , Humanos , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Masculino , Feminino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estudos Longitudinais , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia , Idoso , Área Sob a Curva , Análise Fatorial , Adulto , Atividades Cotidianas/psicologia , Doença Crônica/psicologia , Índice de Massa Corporal
2.
J Intell ; 11(3)2023 Mar 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36976143

RESUMO

The Flynn effect refers to increases over time in measured (particularly fluid) intelligence of approximately 3 IQ points per decade. We define the Flynn effect at the family level, using longitudinal data and two new family-level cohort definitions. Multilevel growth curve analyses of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 data showed that children in families with later-born mothers had higher average PIAT math scores, and lower average reading comprehension scores and growth, in young and middle childhood. Children in families where the first child was born later had higher average PIAT math, reading recognition, and reading comprehension scores, as well as larger developmental growth. The latter family-level Flynn effects were of higher magnitudes than the usual individual-level Flynn effect found in previous studies. Our results, showing family level-intercept and slope Flynn effects for both maternal birthyear and first child birthyear, have implications for research aiming to explain the Flynn effect.

3.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci ; 77(7): 1413-1420, 2022 07 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34244743

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Grip strength is a popular and valuable measure in studies of physical functional capabilities in old age. The influence of historical trends and differential period-specific exposures can complicate the interpretation of biomarkers of aging and health and requires careful analysis and interpretation of aging, birth cohort, and period effects. This study evaluates the effects of aging, period, and cohort on grip strength in a population of adults and older adults. METHODS: We use more than 27 000 observations for individuals at least 50 years of age, born in approximately 1910-1960, from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing to examine a variety of multilevel and cross-classified modeling approaches to evaluate age, period, and cohort effects. Our results extended Hierarchical Age-Period-Cohort modeling and compared our results with a set of 9 submodels with explicit assumptions to determine the most reliable modeling approach. RESULTS: Findings suggest grip strength is primarily related to age, with minimal evidence of either period and/or cohort effects. Each year's increase in a person's age was associated with a 0.40-kg decrease in grip strength, though this decline differs by gender. CONCLUSIONS: We conclude that as the population ages, grip strength declines at a systematic and predictable rate equal to -0.40 kg per year (approximately -0.50 kg for men and -0.30 kg for women) in residents of England aged 50 and older. Age effects were predominant and most consistent across methodologies. While there was some evidence for cohort effects, such effects were minimal and therefore indicative that grip strength is a consistent physiological biomarker of aging.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Força da Mão , Idoso , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Efeito de Coortes , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
4.
Behav Genet ; 51(4): 425-437, 2021 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34089112

RESUMO

Many behavior genetics models follow the same general structure. We describe this general structure and analytically derive simple criteria for its identification. In particular, we find that variance components can be uniquely estimated whenever the relatedness matrices that define the components are linearly independent (i.e., not confounded). Thus, we emphasize determining which variance components can be identified given a set of genetic and environmental relationships, rather than the estimation procedures. We validate the identification criteria with several well-known models, and further apply them to several less common models. The first model distinguishes child-rearing environment from extended family environment. The second model adds a gene-by-common-environment interaction term in sets of twins reared apart and together. The third model separates measured-genomic relatedness from the scanner site variation in a hypothetical functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The computationally easy analytic identification criteria allow researchers to quickly address model identification issues and define novel variance components, facilitating the development of new research questions.


Assuntos
Modelos Genéticos , Gêmeos , Humanos , Gêmeos/genética
5.
Annu Rev Psychol ; 69: 487-510, 2018 01 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29300688

RESUMO

Psychology advances knowledge by testing statistical hypotheses using empirical observations and data. The expectation is that most statistically significant findings can be replicated in new data and in new laboratories, but in practice many findings have replicated less often than expected, leading to claims of a replication crisis. We review recent methodological literature on questionable research practices, meta-analysis, and power analysis to explain the apparently high rates of failure to replicate. Psychologists can improve research practices to advance knowledge in ways that improve replicability. We recommend that researchers adopt open science conventions of preregi-stration and full disclosure and that replication efforts be based on multiple studies rather than on a single replication attempt. We call for more sophisticated power analyses, careful consideration of the various influences on effect sizes, and more complete disclosure of nonsignificant as well as statistically significant findings.


Assuntos
Psicologia , Projetos de Pesquisa , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Estatística como Assunto
6.
J Youth Adolesc ; 44(4): 922-39, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25687264

RESUMO

Better understanding risk factors for the development of adolescent emotional and behavioral problems can help with intervention and prevention efforts. Previous studies have found that an early menarcheal age predicts several adolescent problems, including depressive symptoms, delinquency, and early age at first intercourse. Few studies, nevertheless, have explicitly tested (a) whether the associations with menarcheal age vary across racial/ethnic groups or (b) whether the sources of the associations are within-families (i.e., consistent with a direct, causal link) or only between-families (i.e., due to selection or confounding factors). The current study analyzed data from a nationally representative US Sample of females (N = 5,637). We examined whether race/ethnicity moderated the associations between early menarche and several adolescent problems by using multiple-group analyses and we examined the degree to which genetic and environmental factors shared by family members account for the associations by comparing sisters and cousins with differing menarcheal ages. Menarcheal age predicted subsequent depressive symptoms, delinquency, and early age at first intercourse in the population. The magnitudes of the associations were similar across all racial/ethnic groups for all outcomes. The within-family associations (i.e., when comparing siblings and cousins with different menarcheal age) were large and statistically significant when predicting early intercourse, but not the other outcomes. The findings suggest that selection or confounding factors account for the associations between menarcheal age and subsequent depressive symptoms and delinquency, whereas the independent association between menarcheal age and early age at first intercourse is consistent with a direct, causal effect.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Menarca/fisiologia , Transtornos do Humor/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 121(1): 95-108, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21942334

RESUMO

A quasi-experimental comparison of cousins differentially exposed to levels of neighborhood disadvantage (ND) was used with extensive measured covariates to test the hypothesis that neighborhood risk has independent effects on youth conduct problems (CPs). Multilevel analyses were based on mother-rated ND and both mother-reported CPs across 4-13 years (n = 7,077) and youth-reported CPs across 10-13 years (n = 4,524) from the Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. ND was robustly related to CPs reported by both informants when controlling for both measured risk factors that are correlated with ND and unmeasured confounds. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that ND has influence on conduct problems.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Transtorno da Conduta/psicologia , Características de Residência , Meio Social , Adolescente , Criança , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/epidemiologia , Pré-Escolar , Transtorno da Conduta/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Prevalência , Análise de Regressão , Fatores de Risco , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
9.
Child Dev ; 82(4): 1076-91, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21679176

RESUMO

Nonmaternal care of infant children is increasingly common, but there is disagreement as to whether it is harmful for children. Using data from 9,185 children (5 years and older) who participated in the Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, the current study compared 2 groups: those for whom nonmaternal care was initiated in the first 3 years and those for whom it was not. Between-family comparisons showed that early nonmaternal care was associated with higher achievement and lower behavior problem scores in childhood and adolescence. However, within-family comparisons failed to detect differences between siblings who had different early nonmaternal care experiences. The study concludes that the timing of entry to nonmaternal care in the first 3 years has neither positive nor negative effects on children's outcomes.


Assuntos
Transtornos de Deficit da Atenção e do Comportamento Disruptivo/psicologia , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Cuidado da Criança/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Educação Infantil/psicologia , Irmãos/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Comportamento Materno/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Estados Unidos , Adulto Jovem
10.
Child Dev ; 80(5): 1463-80, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19765012

RESUMO

Children raised without a biological father in the household have earlier average ages of first sexual intercourse than children raised in father-present households. Competing theoretical perspectives have attributed this either to effects of father absence on socialization and physical maturation or to nonrandom selection of children predisposed for early sexual intercourse into father-absent households. Genetically informative analyses of the children of sister dyads (N = 1,382, aged 14-21 years) support the selection hypothesis: This association seems attributable to confounded risks, most likely genetic in origin, which correlated both with likelihood of father absence and early sexual behavior. This holds implications for environmental theories of maturation and suggests that previous research may have inadvertently overestimated the role of family structure in reproductive maturation.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente , Coito , Relações Pai-Filho , Comportamento Sexual , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Família Monoparental , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 50(8): 999-1008, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19490315

RESUMO

Theoretical models concerning how neighborhood contexts adversely influence juvenile antisocial behavior frequently focus on urban neighborhoods; however, previous studies comparing urban and rural areas on the prevalence of youth antisocial behavior have yielded mixed results. The current study uses longitudinal data on the offspring of a nationally representative sample of mothers (N = 4,886) in the US. There was no relation between density and mother-reported child conduct problems across ages 4-13 years, but youth living in areas of greater population density exhibited more youth self-reported delinquency across 10-17 years. Families often moved to counties with greater or lesser population density, but longitudinal analyses treating population density as a time-varying covariate did not support the hypothesis that living in densely populated counties influenced youth delinquency. Rather, the association between population density and delinquency appears to be due to unmeasured selection variables that differ between families who live in more or less densely populated counties.


Assuntos
Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/epidemiologia , Delinquência Juvenil/estatística & dados numéricos , Densidade Demográfica , Características de Residência , Adolescente , Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/diagnóstico , Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/psicologia , Causalidade , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Transtorno da Conduta/diagnóstico , Transtorno da Conduta/epidemiologia , Transtorno da Conduta/psicologia , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Delinquência Juvenil/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Estados Unidos
12.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 116(2): 236-48, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17516757

RESUMO

Sex differences in the causes of self-reported adolescent delinquency were examined in full and half siblings born to a nationally representative sample of women in the United States. Qualitative sex differences in the genes that influence delinquency were not detected. Similarly, the proportions of variance in both aggressive and nonaggressive delinquency attributable to genetic and environmental influences did not differ significantly between girls and boys. Nonetheless, total variance in delinquency was greater among boys, and a scalar sex-differences model suggested that genetic and environmental influences on delinquency have less effect on population variation in delinquency among girls. Similarly, a test of the polygenic multiple threshold model suggested that girls require greater causal liability for the expression of delinquency than boys.


Assuntos
Delinquência Juvenil/estatística & dados numéricos , Autorrevelação , Adolescente , Adulto , Causalidade , Feminino , Predisposição Genética para Doença/genética , Predisposição Genética para Doença/psicologia , Humanos , Delinquência Juvenil/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Modelos Genéticos , Herança Multifatorial/genética , Fenótipo , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Sexuais , Irmãos/psicologia , Meio Social , Estados Unidos
13.
Epidemiology ; 14(1): 60-4, 2003 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12500047

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Numerous rare genetic conditions are known to influence fecundability in both males and females. It is less clear to what extent more subtle genetic differences influence fecundability on a population level. METHODS: In 1994 a population-based survey was conducted among Danish twins born 1953-1982. Fecundability was assessed as the waiting time to pregnancy at the first attempt to achieve a pregnancy. RESULTS: The reported time to pregnancy for males was slightly shorter than for females but there were no sex differences in intrapair similarity. We found an intrapair correlation in time to pregnancy for 645 monozygotic twin pairs (r = 0.22; 95% confidence interval = 0.12 to 0.32), but no intrapair correlation for 826 like-sex dizygotic twin pairs (r = 0.00; 95% confidence interval = -0.09 to 0.10). CONCLUSIONS: The correlation in time to pregnancy for monozygotic twins suggests genetic factors, although similarities in reporting behaviors could also be contributing to the correlation. The lack of correlation in time to pregnancy for dizygotic twins indicates that possible genetic factors of importance for fecundabililty are acting nonadditively. Hence, it may prove difficult to identify specific gene variants that influence fecundability on a population level if their effects depend on gene-gene interactions.


Assuntos
Fertilidade/genética , Gêmeos Dizigóticos/genética , Gêmeos Monozigóticos/genética , Fatores Etários , Dinamarca , Feminino , Variação Genética , Inquéritos Epidemiológicos , Humanos , Masculino , Gravidez , Fatores de Tempo
14.
Psychol Rev ; 109(4): 759-63; discussion 764-71, 2002 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12374330

RESUMO

The Flynn effect is the rise in mean IQ scores during the 20th century, amounting to about 0.33 IQ points per year. Many theoretical explanations have been proposed, though none are universally accepted. W. Dickens and J. R. Flynn's (2001) new approach explains the large IQ changes by means of recursive models of IQ growth. A salient feature of their models is that IQ phenotypes and their supportive environments are correlated; in addition, environmental effects can rebound on phenotypic IQ to increase or lower IQ. In this critique, the authors examine an empirical challenge to their models, which typically imply large changes in IQ variance. However, the historical rise in IQ mean level has not been accompanied by substantial variance changes, a finding inconsistent with the properties of the proposed model.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Inteligência , Modelos Psicológicos , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente , Inteligência/genética , Fatores de Tempo
15.
J Biosoc Sci ; 34(2): 259-75, 2002 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11926458

RESUMO

This study presents a multivariate behavioural genetic analysis of the relationship between education, intelligence and age of first birth. Analyses investigated the mediational role of education in explaining the relationship between intelligence and age of first birth at both the phenotypic and behavioural genetic level. The data come from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY), a nationally representative survey that included genetically informative full- and half-sibling pairs (n = 1423 pairs). Respondents were aged 14 to 22 when contacted in 1979. Heritability estimates were 0.32, 0.50 and 0.06 for IQ, education and age of first birth, respectively. Shared environment estimates were 0.35, 0.23 and 0.20 respectively. Common genetic and shared environmental factors were substantial in explaining the relationship between intelligence and education, and also education and age of first birth. Education partially mediated the relationship between intelligence and age of first birth only in the phenotypic analyses. After considering the genetic and shared environmental factors that influence all three variables, evidence for mediation was less convincing. This pattern of results suggests that the apparent mediational role of education at the phenotypic level is in fact the result of underlying genetic and shared environmental influences that affect education, IQ and age of first birth in common.


Assuntos
Educação/estatística & dados numéricos , Genética Comportamental , Inteligência/genética , Idade Materna , Adolescente , Adulto , Coeficiente de Natalidade/etnologia , Escolaridade , Meio Ambiente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Núcleo Familiar/etnologia , Fenótipo , Gravidez , Distribuição por Sexo , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...