Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Memory ; 9(3): 195-204, 2001 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11469313

RESUMO

At 6 months following the death of their spouse, 37 participants reported their grief-related symptoms and thoughts, and then, approximately 4.5 years later, they attempted to recall how they felt at the time of the 6-month report. Although participants were far less distressed at recall than initially, they recalled their 6-month grief rather accurately. Participants did however overestimate prior grief-related intrusive ideation. Participant's current level of grief predicted what they recalled slightly better than the actual initial grief. Conversely, what they recalled predicted current levels of grief across various measures. Participants whose grief diminished relatively little over time tended to overestimate prior grief more. The retrospective reappraisal that one's past grief was not severe may indicate effective coping.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica/fisiologia , Luto , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Cônjuges , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Psicológicos , Estudos Retrospectivos
2.
Cortex ; 26(3): 409-21, 1990 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2249441

RESUMO

The construct of hemisphericity contends that some individuals process information analytically and others holistically, and that these individual differences reflect characteristic preferences or strategies for left- and right-hemisphere processing, respectively. We investigated whether hemisphericity was related to personality variables, as measured by trait anxiety and the tendency to express versus inhibit emotions. Sixty six right-handed subjects completed personality measures and were tested on two occasions with multiple measures of hemisphericity, including dichotic listening, tachistoscopic emotion recognition, verbal ability and spatial ability. Although some of the hemisphericity measures exhibited moderate reliability, they generally did not correlate significantly with each other or with measures of personality. However, a composite hemisphericity index indicated that right hemisphericity was modestly correlated with the tendency to express emotions and left hemisphericity with the tendency to inhibit emotions. We discuss some reasons for the failure to find either construct or predictive validity for hemisphericity.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral , Psicologia/métodos , Adulto , Cognição , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Inventário de Personalidade , Psicometria
4.
Public Health Rep ; 101(3): 315-9, 1986.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3086926

RESUMO

Cardiac screening programs are ineffective when participants with abnormal findings fail to seek treatment and, to a lesser extent, when participants with normal findings use medical facilities unnecessarily because of continuing concern about heart disease. Age, sex, measure of concern about cardiac symptoms and life stress, and abnormal test results were used to predict the use of physician services in the 3 months following screening. Abnormal test results predicted the use of physician services after screening, as did being older, and having life stress and concern about cardiac symptoms. Being older, male, and concerned about cardiac symptoms predicted having at least one abnormal test result. Participants with normal findings and high levels of concern about cardiac symptoms were as likely to see a physician after the screening as were persons with abnormal findings and low levels of concern about cardiac symptoms. Emphasizing participants' concern about symptoms of heart disease or feelings of stress failed to produce an increase in followup for persons who had abnormal screening outcomes.


Assuntos
Cardiopatias/prevenção & controle , Programas de Rastreamento , Visita a Consultório Médico/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Atitude Frente a Saúde , Feminino , Seguimentos , Hospitais Universitários , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo , Wisconsin
5.
Health Educ Q ; 13(3): 261-80, 1986.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3759479

RESUMO

Two studies assessed the potential impact of health education messages at worksite blood pressure screenings. The messages sought to: motivate hypertensives to enter or return to treatment, motivate normotensives to improve health habits and discourage inappropriate use of blood pressure screening by normotensives. A total of 473 participants in the two studies viewed slide/tape shows about blood pressure and/or health promotion. Individuals with elevated readings at screening viewed either a show containing standard blood pressure information or an experimental show which emphasized the asymptomatic nature of high blood pressure and which described some concrete strategies for coping with high blood pressure. In both studies, individuals with normal readings at screening viewed a standard show or an experimental show which emphasized coping strategies for preventing high blood pressure. In addition, in Study 2, some normotensive individuals viewed one of several experimental shows which focused on health promotion. Results indicate that the experimental programs were significantly more effective than the standard programs in achieving appropriate followup of screening results for both normotensives and hypertensives. Implications for worksite blood pressure screenings are discussed.


Assuntos
Educação em Saúde , Hipertensão/diagnóstico , Serviços de Saúde do Trabalhador , Adulto , Feminino , Educação em Saúde/métodos , Promoção da Saúde , Humanos , Hipertensão/prevenção & controle , Hipertensão/terapia , Masculino , Programas de Rastreamento , Risco
6.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 11(4): 752-63, 1985 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2932524

RESUMO

Forty subjects viewed 10 pictures of facial expressions of emotion while they experienced a happy mood and 10 pictures while they experienced a sad mood. Later, while re-experiencing either a happy or sad mood, they were tested for recognition of these 20 target pictures intermixed with 20 distractors. Recognition of the 10 pictures seen earlier in a disparate mood was impaired significantly when they were presented at testing to the right hemisphere, but not when presented to the left. The right hemisphere appears to store the subject's mood as an integral part of a memory representation for an emotionally expressive face. When that face is presented at testing to the right hemisphere, recognition depends on whether the subject's test mood matches the mood stored in the representation. In contrast, the left hemisphere appears to store the subject's mood separately from encoded visual information about a face, and so recognition of a face presented at testing to the left hemisphere is unaffected by changes in mood.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Afeto/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia
7.
Cortex ; 20(1): 19-25, 1984 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6723325

RESUMO

Subjects judged whether two faces expressed same or different emotions. Subjects were alternately instructed to empathize with or label the first face which was shown for eight seconds. The second face was then exposed briefly in either the left or right visual field. Eighteen subjects had contrasting hemisphere superiorities in accuracy for making same/different judgments under the two instructions, whereas 12 subjects had consistent hemisphere superiorities. It is proposed that individuals may differ in the metacontrol of lateralization on a task, such that for some individuals, the hemisphere controlling cognitive operations can switch as a function of instructions, expectations, and strategies. For other individuals, the metacontrol of lateralization may dictate that the same hemisphere always controls processing on a particular task.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral , Emoções , Expressão Facial , Percepção de Forma , Individualidade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Adulto , Empatia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental
8.
Health Educ Q ; 10(1): 3-29, 1983.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6629788

RESUMO

The models used in the study of communication and health behavior have changed from those describing how to impose health actions on relatively passive respondents to models describing how respondents regulate their own health practices. We have traced the change from the fear-drive model, which described how fear induced change, to the parallel response model, which described how subjects processed information and generated coping responses to solve the problem posed by both the objective health threat and by their subjective fear. The data supporting this change showed that increasing fear led to more favorable attitudes but that fear alone was insufficient to create action: Specific action instructions had to be added to both high and low fear and both combinations produced the same level of health action. Neither the data nor the parallel model specified what subjects learned about the threat that made exposure to a high or low fear message necessary for behavior change. The parallel response model has been elaborated into a more complete systems model and new studies show how health threats are represented. They have found attributes such as IDENTITY (label and symptoms), CAUSES, TIME LINES or duration, and CONSEQUENCES, that set goals and criteria to generate and evaluate problem solving (coping) behavior. Suggestions are made for applying this more complete model to public health practice.


Assuntos
Atitude Frente a Saúde , Educação em Saúde , Modelos Psicológicos , Comportamento , Medo , Humanos , Motivação , Prevenção Primária
9.
Cortex ; 18(4): 569-80, 1982 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7166043

RESUMO

Two experiments tested the hypothesis that positive emotions are processed primarily by the left hemisphere and negative emotions by the right hemisphere. In both experiments, facial expressions of positive and negative emotions were briefly presented to the left and right visual fields of normal dextral subjects. Experiment 1 investigated hemisphere differences in accuracy for naming the emotions, and Experiment 2 examined hemisphere differences in judging the intensity of the emotional expressions. Neither experiment found support for the hypothesized relationship. It was concluded that differential hemisphere involvement in processing positive and negative emotions, as suggested by previous studies, may occur, but only for the production, and not the perception of emotional expressions.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
10.
Public Health Rep ; 97(1): 31-7, 1982.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7058260

RESUMO

The Special Projects Division of the City of Milwaukee Health Department conducted a telephone survey among randomly selected adult residents to determine the population having the greatest potential for benefiting from a health screening a counseling program. A modified version of the Health Hazard Appraisal (HHA) was completed by 268 respondents. From the survey results, it was estimated that by reducing various health risk factors, the respondents could lower their current probability of dying within 10 years by an average of 22 percent. The major predictor of the percentage of reducible risk was age. Persons 18-39 years old could reduce their current probability of dying within 10 years by an average of about 10 percent, whereas persons 40 years and older could reduce theirs by an average of more than 30 percent. Men could lower their probability by slightly more than women, but other sociodemographic factors, such as race, income, education, and residential area, either did not predict significantly the percentage of reducible risk or did so only because of their correlation with age. The results raise questions about the HHA's accuracy in calculating reducible risk, its use of death postponement information to motivate changes in behavior, and its value in health promotion programs, particularly for young adults who are not likely to die of chronic diseases within 10 years. The HHA should only be considered for public health screening programs that target middle-aged and, perhaps, elderly populations rather than the general population of persons under 40 years old.


Assuntos
Inquéritos Epidemiológicos , Expectativa de Vida , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Distribuição Aleatória , Risco , Fatores Sexuais , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Fatores de Tempo , Wisconsin
13.
Med Care ; 17(1): 11-29, 1979 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-759741

RESUMO

Factors affecting delay were studied in patients seeking treatment for the first time for a particular symptom at clinics in a major, innercity hospital. On the basis of the patients' retrospective report, the total time from first noticing a symptom to the seeking of treatment was divided into three sequential stages: 1) appraisal delay--the time the patient takes to appraise a symptom as a sign of illness; 2) illness delay--the time taken from deciding one is ill until deciding to seek professional medical care; and 3) utilization delay--the time from the decision to seek care until the patient goes to the clinic and uses its services. The variables used to predict the length of delay for each of the three stages and for total delay included reports on concrete, sensory perceptions and abstract, conceptual beliefs about one's symptoms, behavioral factors such as strategies for self-appraisal and techniques for coping with illness, emotional reactions, negative imagery elicited by the illness threat, situational barriers, and socio-demographic factors. Patients experiencing a very painful symptom and patients who did not read about their symptom had a short appraisal delay. Patients with old symptoms and those who imagined possible, severe consequences of their illness had long illness delays. Utilization delay was shortest for persons who were not concerned about the cost of treatment, who had a painful symptom, and who were certain that their symptom could be cured. Patients who had short total delays were persons who did not have a competing personal problem and who had a painful symptom. All of these predictors were significantly correlated with the measure of delay at or beyond the p = .01 level. It was concluded that different factors mediate delay in each of the three stages and that studies which use only a single measure of total delay are likely to be of limited value in understanding delay.


Assuntos
Tomada de Decisões , Serviços de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde , Fatores de Tempo , Tempo , Atitude Frente a Saúde , Demografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ambulatório Hospitalar/estatística & dados numéricos , Papel do Doente , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Estados Unidos , População Urbana
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 3(1): 75-82, 1977 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-845557

RESUMO

Two experiments are presented in which lateralization of monaural, auditory input affected the evaluation of verbal passages. In Experiment 1, subjects listened to taped passages that consisted of three levels (positive, neutral, and negative) of tone of voice crossed with three similar levels of content or meaning, making a total of nine different tone/content combinations. Subjects rated these passages as positive, neutral, or negative, and although they were not instructed about which cues (tone or content) to use, 29 of 36 subjects who listened on the left ear primarily used the tone-of-voice cues to rate the passages, whereas 21 of 36 subjects who listened on the right ear used the content cues (p less than .01). In Experiment 2, subjects were asked to make objective ratings of both tone of voice and content. Subjects who listened on the right ear were more accurate in rating both cues. The lateral differences in evaluating the auditory stimuli are discussed in the framework of the left hemisphere's specialization for analytic processing of objective information and the right hemisphere's specialization for the holistic processing of subjective or emotional information. However, each hemisphere's specialization for tone and content cues can be greatly affected by the demands of the experimental task.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Orelha/fisiologia , Emoções , Lateralidade Funcional , Comportamento Verbal , Sinais (Psicologia) , Dominância Cerebral , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Fatores Sexuais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...