Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 2(3): tgab054, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34604753

RESUMO

Anti-saccades are eye movements that require inhibition to stop the automatic saccade to the visual target and to perform instead a saccade in the opposite direction. The inhibitory processes underlying anti-saccades have been primarily associated with frontal cortex areas for their role in executive control. Impaired performance in anti-saccades has also been associated with the parietal cortex, but its role in inhibitory processes remains unclear. Here, we tested the assumption that the dorsal parietal cortex contributes to spatial inhibition processes of contralateral visual target. We measured anti-saccade performance in 2 unilateral optic ataxia patients and 15 age-matched controls. Participants performed 90 degree (across and within visual fields) and 180 degree inversion anti-saccades, as well as pro-saccades. The main result was that our patients took longer to inhibit visually guided saccades when the visual target was presented in the ataxic hemifield and the task required a saccade across hemifields. This was observed through anti-saccades latencies and error rates. These deficits show the crucial role of the dorsal posterior parietal cortex in spatial inhibition of contralateral visual target representations to plan an accurate anti-saccade toward the ipsilesional side.

2.
Cereb Cortex ; 29(9): 3606-3617, 2019 08 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30295717

RESUMO

Attention and saccadic adaptation (SA) are critical components of visual perception, the former enhancing sensory processing of selected objects, the latter maintaining the eye movements accuracy toward them. Recent studies propelled the hypothesis of a tight functional coupling between these mechanisms, possibly due to shared neural substrates. Here, we used magnetoencephalography to investigate for the first time the neurophysiological bases of this coupling and of SA per se. We compared visual discrimination performance of 12 healthy subjects before and after SA. Eye movements and magnetic signals were recorded continuously. Analyses focused on gamma band activity (GBA) during the pretarget period of the discrimination and the saccadic tasks. We found that GBA increases after SA. This increase was found in the right hemisphere for both postadaptation saccadic and discrimination tasks. For the latter, GBA also increased in the left hemisphere. We conclude that oculomotor plasticity involves GBA modulation within an extended neural network which persists after SA, suggesting a possible role of gamma oscillations in the coupling between SA and attention.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Atenção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Ritmo Gama , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia , Masculino
3.
Cortex ; 98: 102-113, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28625347

RESUMO

The concept of peripersonal space was first proposed by Rizzolatti, Scandolara, Matelli, and Gentilucci (1981), who introduced the term to highlight the close links between somatosensory and visual processing for stimuli close to the body and suggested that this near-body space could in fact be characterized as an action space (Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1997). Supporting this idea, patients with right hemisphere lesions have been described as impaired in performing actions towards objects and in perceiving their location - but only when the objects were presented within arm's reach (Bartolo, Carlier, Hassaini, Martin, & Coello, 2014; Brain, 1941). Whether the deficit of optic ataxia patients in processing target locations for action has an effect on the representation of peripersonal space has never been explored. The present study highlights optic ataxia patients' specific difficulties in processing hand-to-target distances in a motor task and in a perceptual task requiring identification of what is reachable in the visual environment. The difficulties are especially evident when both the target and the hand are perceived in the visual periphery. Indeed, when patient I.G. was able to fixate the target, her reaching accuracy and her perception of reachable space both largely improved. Furthermore, the difficulties were enhanced when the hand and the target were both in the lower visual field (in a fixed-far condition vs a fixed-near condition). This novel up-down dimension of optic ataxia fits with the larger representation of the lower visual field in the posterior parietal cortex (Pitzalis et al., 2013; Previc, 1990).


Assuntos
Ataxia/fisiopatologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Julgamento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(6): 2031-2043, 2017 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28039678

RESUMO

Prism adaptation induces rapid recalibration of visuomotor coordination. The neural mechanisms of prism adaptation have come under scrutiny since the observations that the technique can alleviate hemispatial neglect following stroke, and can alter spatial cognition in healthy controls. Relative to non-imaging behavioral studies, fMRI investigations of prism adaptation face several challenges arising from the confined physical environment of the scanner and the supine position of the participants. Any researcher who wishes to administer prism adaptation in an fMRI environment must adjust their procedures enough to enable the experiment to be performed, but not so much that the behavioral task departs too much from true prism adaptation. Furthermore, the specific temporal dynamics of behavioral components of prism adaptation present additional challenges for measuring their neural correlates. We developed a system for measuring the key features of prism adaptation behavior within an fMRI environment. To validate our configuration, we present behavioral (pointing) and head movement data from 11 right-hemisphere lesioned patients and 17 older controls who underwent sham and real prism adaptation in an MRI scanner. Most participants could adapt to prismatic displacement with minimal head movements, and the procedure was well tolerated. We propose recommendations for fMRI studies of prism adaptation based on the design-specific constraints and our results.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Neuroimagem Funcional/instrumentação , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Neuroimagem Funcional/métodos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico por imagem
5.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 9: 426, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26300755

RESUMO

Vertebrates developed sophisticated solutions to select environmental visual information, being capable of moving attention without moving the eyes. A large body of behavioral and neuroimaging studies indicate a tight coupling between eye movements and spatial attention. The nature of this link, however, remains highly debated. Here, we demonstrate that deployment of human covert attention, measured in stationary eye conditions, can be boosted across space by changing the size of ocular saccades to a single position via a specific adaptation paradigm. These findings indicate that spatial attention is more widely affected by oculomotor plasticity than previously thought.

6.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 9: 109, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26283933

RESUMO

Besides its well-known contribution to orienting behaviors, the superior colliculus (SC) might also play a role in controlling visually guided reaching movements. This view has been inferred from studies in monkeys showing that some tectal cells located in the deep layers are active prior to reaching movements; it was corroborated by functional imaging studies performed in humans. Likewise, our group has already demonstrated that, in cats, SC electrical stimulation can modify the trajectory of goal-directed forelimb movements without necessarily affecting the gaze position. However, as in monkeys, we could not establish any congruence between the usual retinotopic SC map and direction of evoked forelimb movements, albeit only a small portion of the collicular map was investigated. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to further ascertain the causal contribution of SC to reaching movement by exploring the whole collicular map. Our results confirmed that SC electrical stimulation deflected the trajectory of reaching movements, but this deviation was always directed downward and backward, irrespective of the location of the stimulation site. The lack of a complete map of reach directions in the SC and the absence of congruence between the direction of evoked forelimb movements and the collicular oculomotor map challenge the view that, in the cat, the SC causally contributes to coding forelimb movements. Interestingly, the very short latencies of the effect argue also against the interruption of reaching movements being driven by a disruption of the early visual processing. Our results rather suggest that the SC might contribute to the reach target selection process. Alternatively, SC stimulation might have triggered a postural adjustment anticipating an upcoming orienting reaction, leading to an interruption of the on-going reaching movement.

8.
J Neurophysiol ; 111(12): 2505-15, 2014 Jun 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24647436

RESUMO

Plastic changes of saccades (i.e., following saccadic adaptation) do not transfer between oppositely directed saccades, except when multiple directions are trained simultaneously, suggesting a saccadic planning in retinotopic coordinates. Interestingly, a recent study in human healthy subjects revealed that after an adaptive increase of rightward-scanning saccades, both leftward and rightward double-step, memory-guided saccades, triggered toward the adapted endpoint, were modified, revealing that target location was coded in spatial coordinates (Zimmermann et al. 2011). However, as the computer screen provided a visual frame, one alternative hypothesis could be a coding in allocentric coordinates. Here, we questioned whether adaptive modifications of saccadic planning occur in multiple coordinate systems. We reproduced the paradigm of Zimmermann et al. (2011) using target light-emitting diodes in the dark, with and without a visual frame, and tested different saccades before and after adaptation. With double-step, memory-guided saccades, we reproduced the transfer of adaptation to leftward saccades with the visual frame but not without, suggesting that the coordinate system used for saccade planning, when the frame is visible, is allocentric rather than spatiotopic. With single-step, memory-guided saccades, adaptation transferred to leftward saccades, both with and without the visual frame, revealing a target localization in a coordinate system that is neither retinotopic nor allocentric. Finally, with single-step, visually guided saccades, the classical, unidirectional pattern of amplitude change was reproduced, revealing retinotopic coordinate coding. These experiments indicate that the same procedure of adaptation modifies saccadic planning in multiple coordinate systems in parallel-each of them revealed by the use of different saccade tasks in postadaptation.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Memória , Desempenho Psicomotor , Movimentos Sacádicos , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Psicofísica
9.
Cereb Cortex ; 24(2): 304-14, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23042755

RESUMO

Sensorimotor adaptation ensures movement accuracy despite continuously changing environment and body. Adaptation of saccadic eye movements is a classical model of sensorimotor adaptation. Beside the well-established role of the brainstem-cerebellum in the adaptation of reactive saccades (RSs), the cerebral cortex has been suggested to be involved in the adaptation of voluntary saccades (VSs). Here, we provide direct evidence for a causal involvement of the parietal cortex in saccadic adaptation. First, the posterior intraparietal sulcus (pIPS) was identified in each subject using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Then, a saccadic adaptation paradigm was used to progressively reduce the amplitude of RSs and VSs, while single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (spTMS) was applied over the right pIPS. The perturbations of pIPS resulted in impairment for the adaptation of VSs, selectively when spTMS was applied 60 ms after saccade onset. In contrast, the adaptation of RSs was facilitated by spTMS applied 90 ms after saccade initiation. The differential effect of spTMS relative to saccade types suggests a direct interference with pIPS activity for the VS adaptation and a remote interference with brainstem-cerebellum activity for the RS adaptation. These results support the hypothesis that the adaptation of VSs and RSs involves different neuronal substrates.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Atividade Motora , Fatores de Tempo , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana
10.
PLoS One ; 8(7): e68560, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23874672

RESUMO

It remains unclear whether spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the mental generation of a visual image (i.e. the arrangement of the component parts of a mental representation). To address this specificity, we recorded eye movements in an imagery task and in a phonological fluency (non-imagery) task, both consisting in naming French towns from long-term memory. Only in the condition of visual imagery the spontaneous eye positions reflected the geographic position of the towns evoked by the subjects. This demonstrates that eye positions closely reflect the mapping of mental images. Advanced analysis of gaze positions using the bi-dimensional regression model confirmed the spatial correlation of gaze and towns' locations in every single individual in the visual imagery task and in none of the individuals when no imagery accompanied memory retrieval. In addition, the evolution of the bi-dimensional regression's coefficient of determination revealed, in each individual, a process of generating several iterative series of a limited number of towns mapped with the same spatial distortion, despite different individual order of towns' evocation and different individual mappings. Such consistency across subjects revealed by gaze (the mind's eye) gives empirical support to theories postulating that visual imagery, like visual sampling, is an iterative fragmented processing.


Assuntos
Cognição , Imagens, Psicoterapia , Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Cerebellum ; 12(4): 557-67, 2013 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23475383

RESUMO

Ataxia with oculomotor apraxia type 2 (AOA2) is one of the most frequent autosomal recessive cerebellar ataxias. Oculomotor apraxia refers to horizontal gaze failure due to deficits in voluntary/reactive eye movements. These deficits can manifest as increased latency and/or hypometria of saccades with a staircase pattern and are frequently associated with compensatory head thrust movements. Oculomotor disturbances associated with AOA2 have been poorly studied mainly because the diagnosis of oculomotor apraxia was based on the presence of compensatory head thrusts. The aim of this study was to characterise the nature of horizontal gaze failure in patients with AOA2 and to demonstrate oculomotor apraxia even in the absence of head thrusts. Five patients with AOA2, without head thrusts, were tested in saccadic tasks with the head restrained or free to move and their performance was compared to a group of six healthy participants. The most salient deficit of the patients was saccadic hypometria with a typical staircase pattern. Saccade latency in the patients was longer than controls only for memory-guided saccades. In the head-free condition, head movements were delayed relative to the eye and their amplitude and velocity were strongly reduced compared to controls. Our study emphasises that in AOA2, hypometric saccades with a staircase pattern are a more reliable sign of oculomotor apraxia than head thrust movements. In addition, the variety of eye and head movements' deficits suggests that, although the main neural degeneration in AOA2 affects the cerebellum, this disease affects other structures.


Assuntos
Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Degenerações Espinocerebelares/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Ataxias Espinocerebelares/congênito , Adulto Jovem
12.
PLoS One ; 8(1): e54641, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23382932

RESUMO

Movement accuracy depends crucially on the ability to detect errors while actions are being performed. When inaccuracies occur repeatedly, both an immediate motor correction and a progressive adaptation of the motor command can unfold. Of all the movements in the motor repertoire of humans, saccadic eye movements are the fastest. Due to the high speed of saccades, and to the impairment of visual perception during saccades, a phenomenon called "saccadic suppression", it is widely believed that the adaptive mechanisms maintaining saccadic performance depend critically on visual error signals acquired after saccade completion. Here, we demonstrate that, contrary to this widespread view, saccadic adaptation can be based entirely on visual information presented during saccades. Our results show that visual error signals introduced during saccade execution--by shifting a visual target at saccade onset and blanking it at saccade offset--induce the same level of adaptation as error signals, presented for the same duration, but after saccade completion. In addition, they reveal that this processing of intra-saccadic visual information for adaptation depends critically on visual information presented during the deceleration phase, but not the acceleration phase, of the saccade. These findings demonstrate that the human central nervous system can use short intra-saccadic glimpses of visual information for motor adaptation, and they call for a reappraisal of current models of saccadic adaptation.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares , Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção Visual , Adaptação Fisiológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
13.
Eur J Neurosci ; 36(5): 2716-21, 2012 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22694116

RESUMO

Involvement of fronto-parietal structures within the right hemisphere in bodily self recognition has gained convergent support from behavioural, neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies. Increases in corticospinal excitability via transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) also testify to right hemisphere self-related processing. However, evidence for self-dependent modulations of motor excitability is limited to the processing of face-related information that, by definition, conveys someone's identity. Here we tested the hypothesis that vision of one's own hand, as compared with vision of somebody else's hand, would also engage specific self-hand processing in the right hemisphere. Healthy participants were submitted to a classic TMS paradigm to assess changes in corticospinal excitability of the right (Experiment 1) and left (Experiment 2) motor cortex, while viewing pictures of a (contralateral) still hand, which could either be their own (Self) or not (Other). As a control for body selectivity, subjects were also presented with pictures of a hand-related, but non-corporeal object, i.e. a mobile phone, which could similarly be their own or not. Results showed a selective right hemisphere increase in corticospinal excitability with self-hand and self-phone stimuli with respect to Other stimuli. Such a Self vs. Other modulation of primary motor cortex appeared at 600 ms and was maintained at 900 ms, but was not present at earlier timings (100 and 300 ms) and was completely absent following stimulation of the left hemisphere. A similar pattern observed for self-hand and self-phone stimuli suggests that owned hands and objects may undergo similar self-processing, possibly via a different cortical network from that responsible for self-face processing.


Assuntos
Ego , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Medula Espinal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Potencial Evocado Motor , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana
14.
Neuroimage ; 61(4): 1100-12, 2012 Jul 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22465298

RESUMO

Potentially dangerous events in the environment evoke automatic ocular responses, called reactive saccades. Adaptation processes, which maintain saccade accuracy against various events (e.g. growth, aging, neuro-muscular lesions), are to date mostly relayed to cerebellar activity. Here we demonstrate that adaptation of reactive saccades also involves cerebral cortical areas. Moreover, we provide the first identification of the neural substrates of adaptation of voluntary saccades, representing the complement to reactive saccades for the active exploration of our environment. An fMRI approach was designed to isolate adaptation from saccade production: an adaptation condition in which the visual target stepped backward 50 ms after saccade termination was compared to a control condition where the same target backstep occurred 500 ms after saccade termination. Subjects were tested for reactive and voluntary saccades in separate sessions. Multi-voxel pattern analyses of fMRI data from previously-defined regions of interests (ROIs) significantly discriminated between adaptation and control conditions for several ROIs. Some of these areas were revealed for adaptation of both saccade categories (cerebellum, frontal cortex), whereas others were specifically related to reactive saccades (temporo-parietal junction, hMT+/V5) or to voluntary saccades (medial and posterior areas of intra-parietal sulcus). These findings critically extend our knowledge on brain motor plasticity by showing that saccadic adaptation relies on a hitherto unknown contribution of the cerebral cortex.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Interpretação de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
15.
PLoS One ; 6(2): e17329, 2011 Feb 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21383849

RESUMO

When goal-directed movements are inaccurate, two responses are generated by the brain: a fast motor correction toward the target and an adaptive motor recalibration developing progressively across subsequent trials. For the saccadic system, there is a clear dissociation between the fast motor correction (corrective saccade production) and the adaptive motor recalibration (primary saccade modification). Error signals used to trigger corrective saccades and to induce adaptation are based on post-saccadic visual feedback. The goal of this study was to determine if similar or different error signals are involved in saccadic adaptation and in corrective saccade generation. Saccadic accuracy was experimentally altered by systematically displacing the visual target during motor execution. Post-saccadic error signals were studied by manipulating visual information in two ways. First, the duration of the displaced target after primary saccade termination was set at 15, 50, 100 or 800 ms in different adaptation sessions. Second, in some sessions, the displaced target was followed by a visual mask that interfered with visual processing. Because they rely on different mechanisms, the adaptation of reactive saccades and the adaptation of voluntary saccades were both evaluated. We found that saccadic adaptation and corrective saccade production were both affected by the manipulations of post-saccadic visual information, but in different ways. This first finding suggests that different types of error signal processing are involved in the induction of these two motor corrections. Interestingly, voluntary saccades required a longer duration of post-saccadic target presentation to reach the same amount of adaptation as reactive saccades. Finally, the visual mask interfered with the production of corrective saccades only during the voluntary saccades adaptation task. These last observations suggest that post-saccadic perception depends on the previously performed action and that the differences between saccade categories of motor correction and adaptation occur at an early level of visual processing.


Assuntos
Movimento/fisiologia , Neurorretroalimentação/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Sensação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Adulto , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
16.
Neuroreport ; 20(10): 913-7, 2009 Jul 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19512951

RESUMO

The portion of space that closely surrounds our body parts is termed peripersonal space, and it has been shown to be represented in the brain through multisensory processing systems. Here, we tested whether voluntary actions, such as grasping an object, may remap such multisensory spatial representation. Participants discriminated touches on the hand they used to grasp an object containing task-irrelevant visual distractors. Compared with a static condition, reach-to-grasp movements increased the interference exerted by visual distractors over tactile targets. This remapping of multisensory space was triggered by action onset and further enhanced in real time during the early action execution phase. Additional experiments showed that this phenomenon is hand-centred. These results provide the first evidence of a functional link between voluntary object-oriented actions and multisensory coding of the space around us.


Assuntos
Força da Mão/fisiologia , Mãos/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Braço/inervação , Braço/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Fenômenos Biomecânicos/fisiologia , Feminino , Mãos/inervação , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Estimulação Física , Tato/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
18.
J Neurophysiol ; 101(3): 1550-9, 2009 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19091922

RESUMO

The accuracy of saccadic eye movements is maintained over the long term by adaptation mechanisms that decrease or increase saccade amplitude. It is still unknown whether these opposite adaptive changes rely on common mechanisms. Here, a double-step target paradigm was used to adaptively decrease (backward second target step) or increase (forward step) the amplitude of reactive saccades in one direction only. To test which sensorimotor transformation stages are subjected to these adaptive changes, we measured their transfer to antisaccades in which sensory and motor vectors are spatially dissociated. In the backward adaptation condition, all subjects showed a significant amplitude decrease for adapted prosaccades and a significant transfer of adaptation to antisaccades performed in the adapted direction, but not to oppositely directed antisaccades elicited by a target jump in the adapted direction. In the forward adaptation condition, only 14 of 19 subjects showed a significant amplitude increase for prosaccades and no significant adaptation transfer to antisaccades was detected in either the adapted or nonadapted direction. These findings suggest that, whereas the level(s) of forward adaptation cannot be resolved, the mechanisms involved in backward adaptation of reactive saccades take place at a sensorimotor level downstream from the vector inversion process of antisaccades and differ markedly from those involved in forward adaptation.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
19.
Exp Brain Res ; 193(3): 337-50, 2009 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19011846

RESUMO

Several processes are devoted to error reduction for the production of purposeful actions. When motor responses deviate from their goal, online corrections can be performed either under voluntary control with additional sub-movements or under fast automatic control with smooth velocity profiles. When errors cannot be corrected online and are repeated over trials, subsequent responses can be improved iteratively through adaptation, a progressive adjustment of motor commands that acts to reduce the magnitude of error. It has been argued that reaching adaptation results essentially from a conflict between actual sensory feedback and expected sensory feedback. Here, we specifically compare two innovative hand-reaching paradigms that provide the subject with undistorted hand sensory feedback. Both paradigms induce motor planning errors unknown to the subjects. Experiment 1 yields a continuous retinal and visuomotor feedback which allows fast and complete automatic online corrections. In experiment 2, all visual feedback is eliminated during movement execution. This prevents online correction and provides information on hand-to-target visual error at movement end only. Despite a reiterated motor planning error and an automatic online correction of the whole error, experiment 1 shows a complete lack of adaptation. In contrast, experiment 2 which yields the same motor planning error exhibits a robust and generalized adaptation, although devoid of limb inter-sensory mismatch. These results demonstrate independence between the induced motor adaptation and automatic online correction, both characterized by the lack of any cognitive interference. Despite these quite different processes acting upon either motor planning or motor control, the general structure of the movement kinematics remains unaltered. A putative visuomotor cerebro-cerebellar network accounting for our results is proposed.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Mãos , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Retroalimentação , Humanos , Movimentos Sacádicos , Visão Ocular
20.
Neuropsychologia ; 46(2): 475-86, 2008 Jan 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17963798

RESUMO

The aim of this work was to investigate ocular control in patients with optic ataxia (OA). Following a lesion in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), these patients exhibit a deficit for fast visuo-motor control of reach-to-grasp movements. Here, we assessed the fast visuo-motor control of saccades as well as spontaneous eye-hand coordination in two bilateral OA patients and five neurologically intact controls in an ecological "look and point" paradigm. To test fast saccadic control, trials with unexpected target-jumps synchronised with saccade onset were randomly intermixed with stationary target trials. Results confirmed that control subjects achieved visual capture (foveation) of the displaced targets with the same timing as stationary targets (fast saccadic control) and began their hand movement systematically at the end of the primary saccade. In contrast, the two bilateral OA patients exhibited a delayed visual capture, especially of displaced targets, resulting from an impairment of fast saccadic control. They also exhibited a peculiar eye-hand coordination pattern, spontaneously delaying their hand movement onset until the execution of a final corrective saccade, which allowed target foveation. To test whether this pathological behaviour results from a delay in updating visual target location, we had subjects perform a second experiment in the same control subjects in which the target-jump was synchronised with saccade offset. With less time for target location updating, the control subjects exhibited the same lack of fast saccadic control as the OA patients. We propose that OA corresponds to an impairment of fast updating of target location, therefore affecting both eye and hand movements.


Assuntos
Ataxia/fisiopatologia , Oftalmopatias/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Ataxia/complicações , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Oftalmopatias/complicações , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Mãos , Humanos , Movimento/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Percepção/complicações , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Valores de Referência
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...