Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Fish Shellfish Immunol ; 33(3): 552-8, 2012 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22771742

RESUMO

Pacific herring (Clupea pallasii) have a central role in the North Pacific ecosystem as a forage fish species and are natural reservoirs of several important finfish pathogens, including Viral hemorrhagic septicemia virus (VHSV). Here, we report the identification of the gene encoding the immunoglobulin mu (IgM) heavy chain, as well as the development and characterization of monoclonal antibodies (MAbs) that specifically react with Pacific herring IgM. Pacific herring immunoglobulin was purified and consisted of heavy and light chains of approximately 80 and 25 kDa. Three hybridoma clones were initially identified by ELISA as reactive with purified immunoglobulin but only one clone was able to detect an 80 kDa protein in Pacific and Atlantic herring (Clupea harengus) whole plasma by denaturing western blot. However, all three MAbs were able to precipitate an 80 kDa protein from Pacific herring and LCMS sequencing of peptide fragments derived from this protein matched the predicted amino acid sequence of the cloned, heavy chain gene. In addition, two of the MAbs stained cells within the putative lymphocyte gates for the spleen, anterior kidney and posterior kidney but were not reactive for myeloid/granulocyte gates, which is consistent with these MAbs reacting with surface IgM⁺ B-cells. To our knowledge, this is the first report of IgM-related gene sequences and anti-IgM monoclonal antibodies from any member of the family Clupeidae. The antibodies produced in this study are critical for achieving our long-term goal of conducting serological surveillance to assess pathogen exposure in natural populations of Pacific herring.


Assuntos
Anticorpos Monoclonais/imunologia , Proteínas de Peixes/imunologia , Peixes/imunologia , Cadeias mu de Imunoglobulina/imunologia , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Animais , Anticorpos Monoclonais/genética , Clonagem Molecular , DNA Complementar/genética , Ensaio de Imunoadsorção Enzimática , Proteínas de Peixes/química , Proteínas de Peixes/genética , Peixes/genética , Perfilação da Expressão Gênica , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Hibridomas/imunologia , Cadeias mu de Imunoglobulina/química , Cadeias mu de Imunoglobulina/genética , Imunoprecipitação , Espectrometria de Massas , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Especificidade de Órgãos , Filogenia , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase em Tempo Real , Alinhamento de Sequência , Análise de Sequência de Proteína
2.
Infect Immun ; 80(8): 2878-85, 2012 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22689811

RESUMO

The interleukin-1 family of cytokines are essential for the control of pathogenic microbes but are also responsible for devastating autoimmune pathologies. Consequently, tight regulation of inflammatory processes is essential for maintaining homeostasis. In mammals, interleukin-1 beta (IL-1ß) is primarily regulated at two levels, transcription and processing. The main pathway for processing IL-1ß is the inflammasome, a multiprotein complex that forms in the cytosol and which results in the activation of inflammatory caspase (caspase 1) and the subsequent cleavage and secretion of active IL-1ß. Although zebrafish encode orthologs of IL-1ß and inflammatory caspases, the processing of IL-1ß by activated caspase(s) has never been examined. Here, we demonstrate that in response to infection with the fish-specific bacterial pathogen Francisella noatunensis, primary leukocytes from adult zebrafish display caspase-1-like activity that results in IL-1ß processing. Addition of caspase 1 or pancaspase inhibitors considerably abrogates IL-1ß processing. As in mammals, this processing event is concurrent with the secretion of cleaved IL-1ß into the culture medium. Furthermore, two putative zebrafish inflammatory caspase orthologs, caspase A and caspase B, are both able to cleave IL-1ß, but with different specificities. These results represent the first demonstration of processing and secretion of zebrafish IL-1ß in response to a pathogen, contributing to our understanding of the evolutionary processes governing the regulation of inflammation.


Assuntos
Caspases/metabolismo , Francisella/classificação , Infecções por Bactérias Gram-Negativas/microbiologia , Inflamação/enzimologia , Interleucina-1beta/metabolismo , Proteínas de Peixe-Zebra/metabolismo , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Animais , Caspases/genética , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Infecções por Bactérias Gram-Negativas/metabolismo , Células HEK293 , Humanos , Interleucina-1beta/genética , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Peixe-Zebra , Proteínas de Peixe-Zebra/genética
3.
Fish Shellfish Immunol ; 32(2): 259-67, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22155011

RESUMO

Infection with the aquatic rhabdovirus Viral hemorrhagic septicemia virus (VHSV) genogroup IVa results in high mortality in Pacific herring (Clupea pallasii) and is hypothesized to be a potential limiting factor for herring recovery. To investigate anti-viral immunity in the Pacific herring, four immune response genes were identified: the myxovirus resistance (Clpa-Mx), a major histocompatibility complex IB (named Clpa-UAA.001), the inducible immunoproteosome subunit 9 (Clpa-PSMB9) and the neutrophil chemotactic factor (Clpa-LECT2). Reverse transcriptase quantitative PCR (RT-qPCR) assays were developed based on these gene sequences to investigate the host immune response to acute VHSV infection following both injection and immersion challenge. Virus levels were measured by both plaque assay and RT-qPCR and peaked at day 6 during the 10-day exposure period for both groups of fish. The interferon stimulated genes (Clpa-Mx, -UAA.001, and -PSMB9) were significantly up-regulated in response to VHSV infection at both 6 and 10 days post-infection in both spleen and fin. Results from this study indicate that Pacific herring mount a robust, early antiviral response in both fin and spleen tissues. The immunological tools developed in this study will be useful for future studies to investigate antiviral immunity in Pacific herring.


Assuntos
Doenças dos Peixes/imunologia , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Septicemia Hemorrágica Viral/imunologia , Nadadeiras de Animais/imunologia , Animais , Doenças dos Peixes/mortalidade , Doenças dos Peixes/virologia , Peixes , Perfilação da Expressão Gênica , Septicemia Hemorrágica Viral/virologia , Novirhabdovirus/imunologia , Baço/imunologia , Carga Viral
4.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24693345

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: By organizing and activating our passions with both hormones and experiences, the heart and mind of sexual behavior, sexual motivation, and sexual preference is the brain, the organ of learning. Despite decades of progress, this incontrovertible truth is somehow lost in the far-too-often biologically deterministic interpretation of genetic, hormonal, and anatomical scientific research into the biological origins of sexual motivation. Simplistic and polarized arguments are used in the media by both sides of the seemingly endless debate over sexual orientation, equality, and human rights with such catch phrases as 'born gay' contrasted against attempts of "reparative therapy" or "pray the gay away". Though long abandoned in practically every other area of psychology, this remnant of the nature-nurture controversy remains despite its generally acknowledged insufficiency in explaining any adult aspect of the human condition within the scientific community. METHODS: THIS THEORETICAL REVIEW ARTICLE IDENTIFIES THREE FACTORS: 1) good intentions with regard to the argument from immutability; 2) false dichotomies limiting intellectual progress by oversimplification of theory and thus hypothesis, and most dangerously, interpretation and; 3) Tradition: a historical separation of the disciplines of biology and psychology, which, to this day, interferes with the effective translation of well-conducted science into good public understanding and policy. RESULTS: Studies clearly demonstrate that progress toward sexual-orientation equality is being made, if slowly, despite the apparent irrelevance of the "born gay" argument from immutability. Evidence is further provided supporting the inadequacy of polarized, dichotic theories of sexual development, particularly those pitting "blank slate learning" against a fated, deterministic biological perspective. Results of this review suggest that an emerging interactionist perspective will promote both better scientific progress and better public understanding, hopefully contributing to progress toward nondiscriminatory public policy. CONCLUSION: Accepting that the brain is a highly plastic, modularly dimorphic, developmentally biased organ of learning, one which is organized and activated by both hormones and experiences across the lifespan, is essential for doing "good science" well. Interactionist theories of psychosexual development provide an empirically sound, strong, yet modifiable foundation for testable hypotheses exploring biologically biased sexual learning.

5.
Fish Shellfish Immunol ; 28(1): 98-105, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19819334

RESUMO

The main objective of this study was to assess correlates of innate resistance in rainbow trout full-sibling families that differ in susceptibility to Infectious hematopoietic necrosis virus (IHNV). As part of a commercial breeding program, full-sibling families were challenged with IHNV by waterborne exposure at the 1 g size to determine susceptibility to IHNV. Progeny from select families (N = 7 families) that varied in susceptibility (ranging from 32 to 90% cumulative percent mortality (CPM)) were challenged again at the 10 g size by intra-peritoneal injection and overall mortality, early viral replication and immune responses were evaluated. Mortality challenges included 20-40 fish per family while viral replication and immune response studies included 6 fish per family at each time point (24, 48 and 72 h post-infection (hpi)). CPM at the 1 g size was significantly correlated with CPM at the 10 g size, indicating that inherent resistance was a stable trait irrespective of size. In the larger fish, viral load was measured by quantitative reverse-transcriptase PCR in the anterior kidney and was a significant predictor of family disease outcome at 48 hpi. Type I interferon (IFN) transcript levels were significantly correlated with an individual's viral load at 48 and 72 hpi, while type II IFN gene expression was significantly correlated with an individual's viral load at 24 and 48 hpi. Mean family type I but not type II IFN gene expression was weakly associated with susceptibility at 72 hpi. There was no association between mean family susceptibility and the constitutive expression of a range of innate immune genes (e.g. type I and II IFN pathway genes, cytokine and viral recognition receptor genes). The majority of survivors from the challenge had detectable serum neutralizing antibody titers but no trend was observed among families. This result suggests that even the most resistant families experienced sufficient levels of viral replication to trigger specific immunity. In summary, disease outcome for each family was determined very early in the infection process and resistance was associated with lower early viral replication.


Assuntos
Vírus da Necrose Hematopoética Infecciosa/imunologia , Oncorhynchus mykiss/imunologia , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/imunologia , Animais , Anticorpos Neutralizantes/imunologia , Anticorpos Antivirais/imunologia , Suscetibilidade a Doenças/imunologia , Regulação da Expressão Gênica/imunologia , Imunidade Inata/imunologia , Interferon Tipo I/biossíntese , Interferon Tipo I/imunologia , Interferon gama/biossíntese , Interferon gama/imunologia , Oncorhynchus mykiss/virologia , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase Via Transcriptase Reversa , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/virologia , Carga Viral/imunologia , Replicação Viral/imunologia
6.
Stress ; 13(1): 36-52, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19658031

RESUMO

The persistent intrusion of remote traumatic memories in people with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may contribute to the impairment of their ongoing hippocampal and prefrontal cortical functioning. In the current work, we have developed a rodent analogue of the intrusive memory phenomenon. We studied the influence of the activation of a remote traumatic memory in rats on their ability to retrieve a newly formed hippocampus-dependent memory. Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats were given inhibitory avoidance (IA) training, and then 24 h or 1, 6 or 12 months later, the same rats were trained to learn, and then remember across a 30-min delay period, the location of a hidden escape platform in the radial-arm water maze (RAWM). When IA-trained rats spent the 30-min delay period in the IA apparatus, they exhibited intact remote (1-year old) memory of the shock experience. More importantly, activation of the rats' memory of the shock experience profoundly impaired their ability to retrieve the newly formed spatial memory of the hidden platform location in the RAWM. Our finding that reactivation of a remote emotional memory exerted an intrusive effect on new spatial memory processing in rats provides a novel approach toward understanding how intrusive memories of traumatic experiences interfere with ongoing cognitive processing in people with PTSD.


Assuntos
Medo/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Eletrochoque , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/fisiologia , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
7.
Fish Shellfish Immunol ; 26(2): 293-304, 2009 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19070666

RESUMO

The genes encoding the type I and type II interferons (IFNs) have previously been identified in rainbow trout and their proteins partially characterized. These previous studies reported a single type II IFN (rtIFN-gamma) and three rainbow trout type I IFN genes that are classified into either group I (rtIFN1, rtIFN2) or group II (rtIFN3). In this present study, we report the identification of a novel IFN-gamma gene (rtIFN-gamma2) and a novel type I group II IFN (rtIFN4) in homozygous rainbow trout and predict that additional IFN genes or pseudogenes exist in the rainbow trout genome. Additionally, we provide evidence that short and long forms of rtIFN1 are actively and differentially transcribed in homozygous trout, and likely arose due to alternate splicing of the first exon. Quantitative reverse transcriptase PCR (qRT-PCR) assays were developed to systematically profile all of the rainbow trout IFN transcripts, with high specificity at an individual gene level, in naïve fish and after stimulation with virus or viral-related molecules. Cloned PCR products were used to ensure the specificity of the qRT-PCR assays and as absolute standards to assess transcript abundance of each gene. All IFN genes were modulated in response to Infectious hematopoietic necrosis virus (IHNV), a DNA vaccine based on the IHNV glycoprotein, and poly I:C. The most inducible of the type I IFN genes, by all stimuli tested, were rtIFN3 and the short transcript form of rtIFN1. Gene expression of rtIFN-gamma1 and rtIFN-gamma2 was highly up-regulated by IHNV infection and DNA vaccination but rtIFN-gamma2 was induced to a greater magnitude. The specificity of the qRT-PCR assays reported here will be useful for future studies aimed at identifying which cells produce IFNs at early time points after infection.


Assuntos
Processamento Alternativo , Duplicação Gênica , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Interferons/genética , Interferons/imunologia , Oncorhynchus mykiss/genética , Oncorhynchus mykiss/imunologia , Adjuvantes Imunológicos/farmacologia , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Animais , Doenças dos Peixes/imunologia , Regulação da Expressão Gênica/efeitos dos fármacos , Vírus da Necrose Hematopoética Infecciosa/imunologia , Interferons/química , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Poli I-C/farmacologia , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase Via Transcriptase Reversa , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/imunologia , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/veterinária , Alinhamento de Sequência , Análise de Sequência de DNA , Homologia de Sequência de Aminoácidos , Vacinas de DNA/farmacologia , Vacinas Virais/farmacologia
8.
Hippocampus ; 16(7): 571-6, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16741974

RESUMO

We have studied the influence of predator stress (30 min of cat exposure) on long-term (24 h) spatial memory and the density of spines in basilar dendrites of CA1 neurons. Predator stress occurred either immediately before water maze training (Stress Pre-Training) or before the 24 h memory test (Stress Pre-Retrieval). The Control (nonstress) group exhibited excellent long-term spatial memory and a robust increase in the density of stubby, but not mushroom, shaped spines. The Stress Pre-Training group had impaired long-term memory and did not exhibit any changes in spine density. The Stress Pre-Retrieval group was also impaired in long-term memory performance, but this group exhibited an increase in the density of stubby, but not mushroom, shaped spines, which was indistinguishable from the control group. These findings indicate that: (1) A single day of water maze training under control conditions produced intact long-term memory and an increase in the density of stubby spines in CA1; (2) Stress before training interfered with the consolidation of information into long-term memory and suppressed the training-induced increase in spine density; and (3) Stress immediately before the 24 h memory test trial impaired the retrieval of the stored memory, but did not reverse the training-induced increase in CA1 spine density. Overall, this work provides evidence of structural plasticity in dendrites of CA1 neurons which may be involved in the consolidation process, and how spinogenesis and memory are modulated by stress.


Assuntos
Espinhas Dendríticas/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia , Amnésia/fisiopatologia , Animais , Gatos , Espinhas Dendríticas/ultraestrutura , Hipocampo/citologia , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Comportamento Predatório , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
9.
Dose Response ; 4(1): 55-74, 2006 Jun 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18648634

RESUMO

The relationship between glucocorticoids (GCs) and memory is complex, in that memory impairments can occur in response to manipulations that either increase or decrease GC levels. We investigated this issue by assessing the relationship between serum corticosterone (the primary rodent GC) and memory in rats trained in the radial arm water maze, a hippocampus-dependent spatial memory task. Each day, rats learned a new location of the hidden escape platform and then 30 min later their memory of the location of the platform was tested. Under control conditions, well-trained rats had excellent spatial memory and moderately elevated corticosterone levels (approximately 26 microg/dl versus a baseline of approximately 2 microg/dl). Their memory was impaired when corticosterone levels were either reduced by metyrapone (a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor) or increased by acute stress (predator exposure), forming an overall U-shaped relationship between corticosterone levels and memory. We then addressed whether there was a causal relationship between elevated corticosterone levels and impaired memory. If elevated corticosterone levels were a sufficient condition to impair memory, then exogenously administered corticosterone, alone, should have impaired performance. However, we found that spatial memory was not impaired in corticosterone-injected rats that were not exposed to the cat. This work demonstrates that an intermediate level of corticosterone correlated with optimal memory, and either a decrease or an increase in corticosterone levels, in conjunction with strong emotionality, impaired spatial memory. These findings indicate that fear-provoking conditions, which are known to engage the amygdala, interact with stress levels of corticosterone to influence hippocampal functioning.

10.
Hippocampus ; 15(8): 1006-25, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16086429

RESUMO

This speculative review serves two purposes. First, it as an extension of the ideas we developed in a previous review (Diamond et al., Hippocampus, 2004;14:281-291), and second, it is a rebuttal to Abraham's (Hippocampus, 2004;14:675-676) critique of that review. We had speculated on the functional significance of the finding that post-training LTP induction produces retrograde amnesia. We noted the similarities between the findings that strong tetanizing stimulation can produce LTP and retrograde amnesia, and that a strong emotional experience can produce a long-lasting memory and retrograde amnesia, as well. The commonalities between LTP induction and emotional learning provided the basis of our hypothesis that an emotional experience generates endogenous LTD/depotentiation, which reverses synaptic plasticity formed during previous learning experiences, and endogenous LTP, which underlies the storage of new information. Abraham raised several concerns with our review, including the criticism that our speculation "falters because there is no evidence that stress causes LTD or depotentiation," and that research on stress and hippocampus has "failed to report any LTP-like changes." Abraham's points are well-taken because stress, in isolation, does not appear to generate long-lasting changes in baseline measures of hippocampal excitability. Here, within the context of a reply to Abraham's critique, we have provided a review of the literature on the influence of stress, novelty, fear conditioning, and the retrieval of emotional memories on cognitive and physiological measures of hippocampal functioning. An emphasis of this review is our hypothesis that endogenous forms of depotentiation, LTD and LTP are generated only when arousing experiences occur in conjunction with memory-related activation of the hippocampus and amygdala. We conclude with speculation that interactions among the different forms of endogenous plasticity underlie a form of competition by synapses and memories for access to retrieval resources.


Assuntos
Amnésia Retrógrada/fisiopatologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Potenciação de Longa Duração/fisiologia , Depressão Sináptica de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Estresse Fisiológico/fisiopatologia , Amnésia Retrógrada/etiologia , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Humanos , Estresse Fisiológico/complicações , Sinapses/fisiologia
11.
Biol Psychiatry ; 57(8): 856-64, 2005 Apr 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15820706

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: There is an extensive literature describing how stress disturbs cognitive processing and can exacerbate psychiatric disorders. There is, however, an insufficient understanding of the molecular mechanisms involved in stress effects on brain and behavior. METHODS: Rats were given spatial memory training in a hippocampus-dependent water maze task. We investigated how a fear-provoking experience (predator exposure) would affect their spatial memory and neural cell adhesion molecule (NCAM) levels in the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala, and cerebellum. RESULTS: Whereas the control (nonstress) group exhibited excellent memory for the hidden platform location in the water maze, the cat-exposed (stress) group exhibited a profound impairment of memory and a marked suppression of levels of the NCAM-180 isoform in the hippocampus. Predator stress produced a more global reduction of NCAM levels in the PFC but had no effect on NCAM levels in the amygdala and cerebellum. CONCLUSIONS: This work provides a novel perspective into dynamic and structure-specific changes in the molecular events involved in learning, memory, and stress. The selective suppression of NCAM-180 in the hippocampus and the more general suppression of NCAM in the PFC provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the great sensitivity of these two structures to be disturbed by stress.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/metabolismo , Moléculas de Adesão de Célula Nervosa/biossíntese , Córtex Pré-Frontal/metabolismo , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/metabolismo , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Animais , Western Blotting , Química Encefálica/fisiologia , Gatos , Corticosterona/sangue , Medo/fisiologia , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/fisiologia , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
12.
Hippocampus ; 14(3): 281-91, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15132427

RESUMO

Models of the neurobiology of memory have been based on the idea that information is stored as distributed patterns of altered synaptic weights in neuronal networks. Accordingly, studies have shown that post-training treatments that alter synaptic weights, such as the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP), can interfere with retrieval. In these studies, LTP induction has been relegated to the status of a methodological procedure that serves the sole purpose of disturbing synaptic activity in order to impair memory. This perspective has been expressed, for example, by Martin and Morris (2002: Hippocampus 12:609-636), who noted that post-training LTP impairs memory by adding "behaviorally meaningless" noise to hippocampal neural networks. However, if LTP truly is a memory storage mechanism, its induction should represent more than just a means with which to disrupt memory. Since LTP induction produces retrograde amnesia, the formation of a new memory should also produce retrograde amnesia. In the present report, we suggest that one type of learning experience, the storage of fear-related (i.e., stressful) memories, is consistent with this prediction. Studies have shown that stress produces potent effects on hippocampal physiology, generates long-lasting memories, and induces retrograde amnesia, all through mechanisms in common with LTP. Based on these findings, we have developed the hypothesis that a stressful experience generates an endogenous form of hippocampal LTP that substitutes a new memory representation for preexisting representations. In summary, our hypothesis implicates the induction of endogenous synaptic plasticity by stress in the formation of emotional memories and in retrograde amnesia.


Assuntos
Amnésia Retrógrada/fisiopatologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiopatologia , Potenciação de Longa Duração/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Estresse Fisiológico/fisiopatologia , Amnésia Retrógrada/etiologia , Animais , Medo/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos
13.
Learn Mem ; 10(5): 326-36, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14557605

RESUMO

We have shown previously that psychological stress (predator exposure) impairs spatial memory in rats. We have extended that finding here to show that predator stress selectively impaired recently acquired (hippocampal-dependent) spatial working memory without affecting long-term (hippocampal-independent) spatial reference memory. We also investigated why predator exposure impairs memory. Was spatial memory impaired because of the fear-provoking aspects of predator exposure or only because the cat was a novel and arousing stimulus? If the latter possibility was correct, then any novel and arousing stimulus, independent of its emotional valence (i.e., aversive or appetitive), would impair memory. We found that spatial working memory was not impaired when the male rats were exposed to a sexually receptive female rat, a stimulus that was novel and arousing to them, but not aversive. We also found that there was an equivalent increase in serum corticosterone levels in male rats exposed to either a cat or a female rat, but only the cat-exposed rats exhibited a significant correlation between corticosterone levels and impaired memory. Overall, this series of experiments demonstrates that (1). predator stress selectively impaired working (hippocampal-dependent), but not reference (hippocampal-independent), memory; (2). a fear-provoking stimulus, and not merely novelty and increased arousal, impaired spatial memory; and (3). increased corticosterone levels correlated with impaired spatial working memory only under predator exposure, that is, fear-provoking conditions.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Corticosterona/sangue , Corticosterona/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico , Amnésia/fisiopatologia , Animais , Emoções/fisiologia , Medo/psicologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia
14.
Horm Behav ; 42(2): 148-57, 2002 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12367568

RESUMO

A three-phase experiment manipulated sexual experience and hormone exposure (perinatally and in adulthood) in female rats housed individually from weaning so as to limit peripubertal social and sexual experience. Noncontact partner preference for a male or estrous female rat was measured both before and after sexual experience, first while rats were under the influence of circulating testosterone propionate (TP) and later after priming them with ovarian hormones (estradiol benzoate and progesterone; EB & P). When implanted with TP capsules and tested while sexually naive, all groups of female rats preferred females to males without differing statistically. However, following three sexual experience sessions with estrous females, differences emerged between the masculinized and control groups in the magnitude of their female-directed preference, with masculinized females demonstrating a significantly greater preference for estrous females. Sexual experience with male rats under EB & P did not result in a significant shift in preference in any group. Histological assessment indicated that the volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) was increased by exposure to TP postnatally, and SDN-POA volume correlated positively with partner preference scores but only when rats were both sexually experienced and exposed to circulating TP in adulthood. These results suggest that sexual experience interacts with steroid exposure to shape partner preference.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/farmacologia , Comportamento Sexual Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Sinergismo Farmacológico , Estradiol/farmacologia , Feminino , Masculino , Ovariectomia , Área Pré-Óptica/anatomia & histologia , Progesterona/farmacologia , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Caracteres Sexuais , Testosterona/farmacologia , Virilismo/induzido quimicamente , Virilismo/psicologia
15.
Q J Exp Psychol B ; 55(1): 75-88, 2002 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11900308

RESUMO

Male and female rats were gonadectomized, implanted in adulthood with capsules containing either testosterone propionate (TP) or cholesterol, and were trained to lever press for access to an oestrous female. When lever press performance was tested in extinction, only the male rats implanted with TP displayed significantly higher levels of responding than controls, demonstrating that lever pressing for oestrous females as a reward is sexually dimorphic. Ejaculation patterns from a separate assessment of these rats' copulatory ability were significantly correlated with their instrumental performance. In a second experiment, "masculinized" females exposed to TP postnatally and given TP implants responded in extinction at mean levels equivalent to those exhibited by adult males that were either intact or castrated with androgen replacement. These data suggest that organizational steroid exposure perinatally affects the actual reward value assigned to oestrous females in adulthood, in combination with consummatory sexual experience.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Recompensa , Comportamento Sexual Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Castração , Copulação/efeitos dos fármacos , Ejaculação/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Caracteres Sexuais , Testosterona/farmacologia , Fatores de Tempo
16.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 26(1): 69-80, 2002 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11835985

RESUMO

Learning plays numerous important roles in sexual development. Yet, the possible impacts on sexuality, of learning from experience, are rarely included in discussions of the organization of behavioral sex differences and the differentiation of psychosexual function. This article reviews the empirical evidence for 'learned sexuality' with a goal of reintroducing the topic of nurture into discussions of the ontogenetic processes that lead to sexual reproduction in nature. Evolutionarily relevant examples of sexual learning are broadly represented in the animal kingdom, and can occur relatively early in development, leading to lasting changes in behaviors that might otherwise appear to be instinctive, or in other cases, maladaptive. The lasting effects of social and sexual experiences across the lifespan provide an essential link between steroid-mediated events occurring during development, behavioral plasticity, and changes in motivational states in adulthood.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Maturidade Sexual/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos , Meio Social
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...