Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 49
Filtrar
1.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 42(6): 767-76, 2001 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11583249

RESUMO

Handedness preference was assessed in 205 boys with gender identity disorder and 205 clinical control boys referred for other reasons. Boys with gender identity disorder were significantly more likely to be left-handed than the clinical control boys (19.5% vs. 8.3%, respectively). The boys with gender identity disorder, but not the clinical control boys, also had a significantly higher rate of left-handedness compared to three independent, general population studies of nonreferred boys (11.8%; N = 14,253) by Hardyck, Goldman, and Petrinovich (1975), Calnan and Richardson (1976), and Eaton, Chipperfield, Ritchot, and Kostiuk (1996). Left-handedness appears to be a behavioral marker of an underlying neurobiological process associated with gender identity disorder in boys.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Identidade de Gênero , Transtornos Sexuais e da Identidade de Gênero/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Masculino , Estudos de Amostragem , Caracteres Sexuais
3.
Arch Sex Behav ; 29(5): 463-78, 2000 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10983250

RESUMO

Whether homosexual pedophiles have more older brothers (a higher fraternal birth order) than do heterosexual pedophiles was investigated. Subjects were 260 sex offenders (against children age 14 or younger) and 260 matched volunteer controls. The subject's relative attraction to male and female children was assessed by phallometric testing in one analysis, and by his offense history in another. Both methods showed that fraternal birth order correlates with homosexuality in pedophiles, just as it does in men attracted to physically mature partners. Results suggest that fraternal birth order (or the underlying variable it represents) may prove the first identified universal factor in homosexual development. Results also argue against a previous explanation of the high prevalence of homosexuality in pedophiles (25% in this study), namely, that the factors that determine sexual preference in pedophiles are different from those that determine sexual preference in men attracted to adults. An alternative explanation in terms of canalization of development is suggested.


Assuntos
Ordem de Nascimento , Pedofilia/epidemiologia , Comportamento Sexual , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Homossexualidade Masculina/psicologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pedofilia/psicologia , Ereção Peniana/fisiologia , Fatores Sexuais
4.
Arch Sex Behav ; 29(4): 349-56, 2000 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10948724

RESUMO

The birth order of a contemporary North American sample of 97 gay men was quantified using Slater's Index. For the 84 probands with at least one sibling, the results showed a late mean birth order compared with the expected value of .50. Additional birth order indices derived from Slater's Index suggested that the mean later birth order was accounted for more strongly by the proband's number of older brothers than by his number of older sisters. The present findings constitute a replication of a series of recent studies and add to the growing body of evidence that birth order is a reliable correlate of sexual orientation in males.


Assuntos
Ordem de Nascimento , Homossexualidade Masculina/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Comportamento Sexual
5.
Psychol Bull ; 126(4): 575-92, 2000 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10900997

RESUMO

Recent findings suggest that sexual orientation has an early neurodevelopmental basis. Handedness, a behavioral marker of early neurodevelopment, has been associated with sexual orientation in some studies but not in others. The authors conducted a meta-analysis of 20 studies that compared the rates of non-right-handedness in 6,987 homosexual (6,182 men and 805 women) and 16,423 heterosexual (14,808 men and 1,615 women) participants. Homosexual participants had 39% greater odds of being non-right-handed. The corresponding values for homosexual men (20 contrasts) and women (9 contrasts) were 34% and 91%, respectively. The results support the notion that sexual orientation in some men and women has an early neurodevelopmental basis, but the factors responsible for the handedness-sexual orientation association require elucidation. The authors discuss 3 possibilities: cerebral laterality and prenatal exposure to sex hormones, maternal immunological reactions to the fetus, and developmental instability.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/embriologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Dominância Cerebral/genética , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Homossexualidade , Encéfalo/imunologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/genética , Homossexualidade/genética , Humanos , Masculino , Troca Materno-Fetal , Plasticidade Neuronal , Gravidez , Efeitos Tardios da Exposição Pré-Natal , Caracteres Sexuais
9.
Arch Sex Behav ; 28(6): 475-502, 1999 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10650437

RESUMO

Gender constancy judgments in children referred for problems in their gender identity development (N = 206) and controls (N = 95) were compared. On Slaby and Frey's (1975) gender constancy interview, the gender-referred children performed more poorly than the controls at three stage levels: gender identity, gender stability, and gender consistency. On the Boy-Girl Identity Task, a second measure of gender constancy (Emmerich et al., 1977), the gender-referred children also performed more poorly. Gender-referred children who had not attained gender consistency engaged in significantly less same-sex-typed play on a free-play task than the gender-referred children who had, but there were no gender consistency effects for the controls. Two other measures of sex-typed behavior were unrelated to gender consistency. In the gender-referred group alone, children who "failed" the gender identity or gender stability stages were more likely to draw an opposite-sex person first on the Draw-a-Person test and to evince more affective gender confusion on the Gender Identity Interview (Zucker et al., 1993) than children who had "passed." It is concluded that children referred for problems in their gender identity development have a developmental lag in gender constancy acquisition. Possible reasons for the lag are discussed.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/diagnóstico , Deficiências do Desenvolvimento/diagnóstico , Identidade de Gênero , Julgamento/fisiologia , Criança , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/complicações , Estudos Transversais , Deficiências do Desenvolvimento/complicações , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudos Retrospectivos , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia
10.
Can J Psychiatry ; 43(10): 1040-3, 1998 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9868571

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To test the hypothesis that adoptees are overrepresented among a sample of clinic-referred boys with gender identity problems (N = 238). To compare the adoptees and nonadoptees on demographic, behaviour problem, and gender-typed measures. METHOD: The percentage of clinic-referred boys with gender identity problems adopted in the first 2 years of life ("early adoptees") was compared to the base rate of boys adopted in Ontario. Parent-report and behavioural measures were used to compare the early adoptees with "late adoptees" (adopted after the second year of life) and nonadoptees. RESULTS: The percentage of boys with gender identity problems who were early adoptees (7.6%) was significantly higher than the base rate of males adopted in Ontario in the first 2 years of life (1.5%). Both the early and late adoptees were significantly less intelligent than the nonadoptees. The early adoptees also had significantly higher externalizing T scores on the Child Behavior Checklist than did the late adoptees and the nonadoptees. The 3 groups did not differ in the percentage who met the complete Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) criteria for gender identity disorder and on 4 other measures of gender-typed behaviour. CONCLUSION: Adoptees are overrepresented among clinic-referred boys with gender identity problems. The reasons for this finding are not clear but may be accounted for by general risk factors that increase the likelihood of clinical referral or by psychosocial and biological factors associated with adoption.


Assuntos
Adoção/psicologia , Identidade de Gênero , Encaminhamento e Consulta , Fatores Etários , Criança , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/epidemiologia , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Ontário/epidemiologia , Determinação da Personalidade , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Encaminhamento e Consulta/estatística & dados numéricos , Fatores de Risco
11.
J Biosoc Sci ; 30(4): 511-9, 1998 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9818557

RESUMO

Homosexual men have a higher mean birth order than do heterosexual men, primarily because they have a greater number of older brothers. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the same difference occurs in homosexual vs heterosexual women. The probands were 964 homosexual and heterosexual, male and female volunteers, from whom birth order data were collected with self-administered questionnaires. The homosexual men had more older brothers than the heterosexual men, but they did not have more older sisters, younger brothers, or younger sisters. The homosexual women did not differ from the heterosexual women with regard to any class of sibling. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that the high birth order of homosexual men reflects the progressive immunization of certain mothers to H-Y antigen by succeeding male fetuses, and the increasing effects of H-Y antibodies on sexual differentiation of the brain in succeeding male fetuses.


Assuntos
Ordem de Nascimento , Homossexualidade Feminina/estatística & dados numéricos , Homossexualidade Masculina/estatística & dados numéricos , Comportamento Sexual , Adulto , Coleta de Dados , Características da Família , Feminino , Humanos , Londres , Masculino
12.
Pediatrics ; 102(1): e9, 1998 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9651461

RESUMO

Guidelines of psychosexual management for infants born with physical intersex conditions are intended to assist physicians and parents in making decisions about sex of assignment and rearing including the following: 1) sex assignment should be to the gender that carries the best prognosis for good reproductive function, good sexual function, normal-looking external genitalia and physical appearance, and a stable gender identity; 2) the decision regarding sex assignment should be made as early as possible, preferably during the newborn period, with an upper age limit for reversal of an initial sex assignment no later than 18 to 24 months; and 3) there should be minimal uncertainty and ambiguity on the part of parents and professionals regarding the final decision about sex assignment and rearing. J. Money used these guidelines in a case of a biologically normal male infant (one of a pair of monozygotic twins) whose penis was accidentally ablated during a circumcision at the age of 7 months. The decision to reassign the infant boy to the female sex and to rear him as a girl was made at 17 months, with surgical castration and initial genital reconstruction occurring at 21 months. Money reported follow-up data on this child through the age of 9 years. Although the girl was described as having many "tomboyish" behavioral traits, a female gender identity had apparently differentiated. Thus, it was concluded that gender identity is sufficiently incompletely differentiated at birth as to permit successful assignment of a genetic male as a girl, in keeping with the experiences of rearing. Subsequent follow-up by other investigators reported that by early adolescence the patient had rejected the female identity and began to live as a male at the age of 14 years. In adulthood, the patient recalled that he had never felt comfortable as a girl, and his mother reported similar recollections. At age 25, the patient married a woman and adopted her children. The patient reported exclusive sexual attraction to females. The present case report is a long-term psychosexual follow-up on a second case of ablatio penis in a 46 XY male. During an electrocautery circumcision at the age of 2 months, the patient sustained a burn of the skin of the entire penile shaft, and the penis eventually sloughed off. At age 7 months, the remainder of the penis and the testes were removed. By age 7 months, if not earlier, the decision was made to reassign the patient as a female and to raise the infant as a girl. The patient was interviewed on two occasions: at 16 years and twice while in the hospital for additional surgery at 26 years of age. At ages 16 and 26, the patient was living socially as a woman and denied any uncertainty about being a female. During childhood, the patient recalled that she self-identified as a "tomboy" and enjoyed stereotypically masculine toys and games; however, the patient also recalled that her favorite playmates were usually girls and that her best friend was always a girl. When seen at age 16, the patient had been admitted to the hospital for vaginoplasty. At that time, she wished to proceed with the further repair of her genitalia to make them suitable for sexual intercourse with males. At age 26, the patient returned to the hospital for further vaginoplasty. Regarding the patient's sexual orientation, she was attracted predominantly to women in fantasy, but had had sexual experiences with both women and men. At the time of the second surgery, she was in a relationship with a man and wished to be able to have intercourse. The patient's self-described sexual identity was "bisexual." After surgery at age 26 years, the patient developed a rectovaginal fistula. Within a few months of the surgery, the patient and her male partner separated for reasons other than the patient's physical problems. The patient subsequently began living with a new partner, a woman, in a lesbian relationship. The psychosexual development of our patient was bot the other patient was married to a woman. Our patient had a "bisexual" sexual identity; the other patient had a "heterosexual" sexual identity. The patients were similar in that they had a childhood history of "tomboyism." Our patient was predominantly sexually attracted to women; the other patient was exclusively sexually attracted to women. Our patient had sexual experiences with both women and men; the other patient had sexual experiences only with women. The most plausible explanation of our patient's differentiation of a female gender identity is that sex of rearing as a female, beginning at around age 7 months, overrode any putative influences of a normal prenatal masculine sexual biology. Because cases of ablatio penis in infancy are so rare and long-term follow-up data are scant, it is obviously impossible to know whether our patient or the previous case would be more typical of the psychosexual outcome in a larger sample of such individuals. However, our case suggests that it is possible for a female gender identity to differentiate in a biologically "normal" genetic male. At present, however, the clinical literature is deeply divided on the best way to manage cases of traumatic loss of the penis during infancy. Further study is clearly required to decide on the optimal model of psychosocial and psychosexual management for affected individuals.


Assuntos
Amputação Traumática/reabilitação , Identidade de Gênero , Pênis/lesões , Bissexualidade/psicologia , Circuncisão Masculina/efeitos adversos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento Sexual , Seguimentos , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuais
13.
Arch Sex Behav ; 27(3): 253-67, 1998 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9604115

RESUMO

University students, masked to group status, rated the physical appearance of boys and girls with gender identity disorder (GID) and same-sex controls using traits with stereotypical masculine connotations (for boys: all-boy, handsome, masculine, and rugged; for girls: handsome, masculine, rugged, and tomboyish). Three traits (all-boy, masculine, and rugged) were judged to be significantly less characteristic of the boys with GID as compared to the same-sex controls; for the fourth trait (handsome), the inverse result was obtained. All four traits (handsome, masculine, rugged, and tomboyish) were judged to be significantly more characteristic of the girls with GID as compared to the same-sex normal and/or clinical controls. These results were the inverse of the results obtained in two previous experiments, in which traits with stereotypical feminine connotations were used (Fridell et al., 1996; Zucker et al., 1993). Taken together, the results suggest that boys and girls with GID have a sex-typed physical appearance that distinguishes them from same-sex controls. Possible determinants of these differences are discussed.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Identidade de Gênero , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/psicologia , Adulto , Criança , Estética , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estereotipagem
14.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 7(1): 30-5, 1998 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9563811

RESUMO

This study examined the birth order of girls with gender identity disorder (N = 22). Each proband was matched to 3-7 clinical control girls for age at assessment and number of siblings (the mode number of controls per proband was 7) (total N = 147). The number of older brothers, older sisters, younger brothers, and younger sisters was recorded. Slater's birth order index showed that the probands were significantly more likely to be early born than were the controls. A modified Slater's index also compared the birth order of the probands and the controls only to their brothers (when they had one or more) and only to their sisters (when they had one or more). Compared to the controls, the probands were born early compared to their sisters, but not to their brothers. These findings are the inverse of two previous studies of boys with gender identity disorder, who were later born relative to clinical control boys, an effect that appeared to be accounted for primarily by being born later relative to older brothers, but not to older sisters.


Assuntos
Ordem de Nascimento/psicologia , Características da Família , Relações entre Irmãos , Transexualidade/etiologia , Transexualidade/psicologia , Adolescente , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Identidade de Gênero , Humanos , Pais/psicologia , Encaminhamento e Consulta , Sexo , Temperamento , Transexualidade/diagnóstico
15.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 36(7): 872-80, 1997 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9204664

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To review the clinically relevant literature on gender identity disorder (GID) in children and adolescents over the past 10 years. METHOD: All literature referring to gender identity and children or adolescents from 1985 on was reviewed. RESULTS: Changes in the DSM-IV nomenclature include (1) adoption of the single diagnosis of GID to apply to children, adolescents, and adults; (2) changes in the format of the criteria; and (3) placement in the section "Sexual and Gender Identity Disorders." Rates of associated psychopathology in children with GID are comparable with those in children with other psychiatric disorders, particularly disorders that are internalizing in form. Biological and psychosocial factors thought to be relevant in the development of GID are reviewed. CONCLUSIONS: Research is required to elucidate the complicated interaction between biological and psychosocial factors in the development of GID and to evaluate treatment efficacy.


Assuntos
Identidade de Gênero , Desenvolvimento Psicossexual , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/diagnóstico , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/classificação , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/psicologia
16.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 38(5): 543-51, 1997 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9255698

RESUMO

Sibling sex ratio (the ratio of brothers to sisters) was calculated for 444 boys with gender identity disorder (or with behaviors consistent with this diagnosis). The probands were ascertained from several researchers with expertise with this disorder and from the English language case report literature between 1938 and 1995. Among the probands with at least one sibling (N = 333), the results showed that boys with gender identity disorder had a significant excess of brothers to sisters, 131.1:100, when compared with the expected secondary sex ratio of 106:100. The excess of brothers replicated a previous study by Blanchard, Zucker, Bradley, and Hume (1995), in which the sibling sex ratio was 140.6:100. Further analyses showed that the probands were born later relative to their brothers than they were relative to their sisters. These findings are amenable to several psychosocial and biological explanations, which require further investigation.


Assuntos
Identidade de Gênero , Desenvolvimento Psicossexual , Razão de Masculinidade , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/genética , Relações entre Irmãos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Características da Família , Humanos , Masculino
17.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 25(3): 217-27, 1997 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9212374

RESUMO

From 1978 through 1995, a sex ratio of 6.6:1 of boys to girls (N = 275) was observed for children referred to a specificity clinic for gender identity disorder. This article attempts to evaluate several hypotheses regarding the marked sex disparity in referral rates. The sexes did not differ on four demographic variables (age at referral, IQ, and parent's social class and marital status) and on five indices of general behavior problems on the Child Behavior Checklist; in addition, there was only equivocal evidence that boys with gender identity disorder had significantly poorer peer relations than girls with gender identity disorder. Although the percentage of boys and girls who met the complete DSM-III-R criteria for gender identity disorder was comparable, other measures of sex-typed behavior showed that the girls had more extreme cross-gender behavior than the boys. Coupled with external evidence that cross-gender behavior is less tolerated in boys than in girls by both peers and adults, it is concluded that social factors partly account for the sex difference in referral rates. Girls appear to require a higher threshold than boys for cross-gender behavior before they are referred for clinical assessment.


Assuntos
Identidade de Gênero , Pais/psicologia , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde/etnologia , Encaminhamento e Consulta , Transexualidade/psicologia , Análise de Variância , Atitude Frente a Saúde/etnologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Comorbidade , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Ontário/epidemiologia , Seleção de Pacientes , Grupo Associado , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Fatores Sexuais , Percepção Social , Estereotipagem , Transexualidade/epidemiologia
19.
Arch Sex Behav ; 26(6): 671-3, 1997 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9415800
20.
Horm Behav ; 30(4): 300-18, 1996 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9047259

RESUMO

Women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) (N = 31) and their unaffected sisters or female cousins (N = 15) participated in a study of psychosexual development. All participants were > or = 18 years of age (mean age, 25 years; range, 18-40). Comparisons were also made between the CAH women with the salt-wasting (SW) form of the disorder and those with simple virilization (SV). A psychosexual assessment protocol examined six variables: (1) sex assignment at birth (probands only); (2) recalled sex-typed behavior during childhood; (3) gender identity and gender role identification in adulthood; (4) relationship status; (5) sexual orientation in fantasy; and (6) sexual orientation in behavior. Salt-wasting status and sex assignment at birth were also ascertained for the CAH women who either refused to participate in the study (N = 10) or could not be traced (N = 13). Compared to the controls, the women with CAH recalled more cross-gender role behavior and less comfort with their sense of "femininity" during childhood. The two groups did not differ in degree of gender dysphoria in adulthood, although the probands showed more cross-gender role identification. Three of the nonparticipant probands were living, as adults, in the male social role (2 reared from birth as boys and 1 who changed from the female to the male social role during adolescence). The CAH women and the controls did not differ in relationship status (married/cohabiting vs. single). The CAH women had lower rates of exclusive heterosexual fantasy and fewer sexual experiences with men than the controls; however, the CAH women did not have more sexual experiences with women than the controls. Comparisons between the SW and SV revealed several differences: the SW were less likely to be assigned to the female sex at birth, recalled more cross-gender role behavior during childhood, were less likely to be married or cohabiting, and had lower rates of sexual experiences with men. The results were discussed in relation to the effects of prenatal androgens on psychosexual differentiation.


Assuntos
Hiperplasia Suprarrenal Congênita/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Psicossexual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA