Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 15.654
Filtrar
1.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 33, 2024 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38771569

RESUMO

Purpose: This study explored early (contrast discrimination) and intermediate (global form perception) visual processing in primary subtypes of glaucoma: primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) and primary angle-closure glaucoma (PACG). We aimed to understand early and intermediate visual processing in POAG and PACG, matched for similar visual field defect severity. Methods: Early visual processing was measured using a contrast discrimination task described by Porkorny and Smith (1997), and intermediate processing using a global form perception task using glass pattern coherence thresholds. Thresholds were determined centrally and at a single midperipheral location (12.5°) in a quadrant without visual field defects. Controls were tested in corresponding quadrants to individuals with glaucoma. Results: Sixty participants (20 POAG, 20 PACG, and 20 age-matched controls), aged 50 to 77 years, were included. Visual field defects were matched between POAG and PACG, with mean deviation values of -6.53 ± 4.46 (range: -1.5 to -16.85) dB and -6.2 ± 4.24 (range: -1.37 to -16.42) dB, respectively. Two-Way ANOVA revealed significant differences in thresholds between the glaucoma groups and the control group for both contrast discrimination and global form perception tasks, with higher thresholds in the glaucoma groups. Post hoc analyses showed no significant contrast discrimination difference between POAG and PACG, but POAG had significantly higher thresholds than PACG for form perception. Conclusions: In form perception, POAG showed slightly worse performance than PACG, suggesting that individuals with POAG may experience more severe functional damage than PACG of similar visual field severity.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste , Percepção de Forma , Glaucoma de Ângulo Fechado , Glaucoma de Ângulo Aberto , Campos Visuais , Humanos , Glaucoma de Ângulo Aberto/fisiopatologia , Glaucoma de Ângulo Fechado/fisiopatologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso , Masculino , Feminino , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Pressão Intraocular/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Testes de Campo Visual
2.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(4): 13, 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573617

RESUMO

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to assess motion-defined form perception, including the association with clinical and sensory factors that may drive performance, in each eye of children with deprivation amblyopia due to unilateral cataract. Methods: Coherence thresholds for orientation discrimination of motion-defined form were measured using a staircase procedure in 30 children with deprivation amblyopia and 59 age-matched controls. Visual acuity, stereoacuity, fusion, and interocular suppression were also measured. Fixation stability and fellow-eye global motion thresholds were measured in a subset of children. Results: Motion-defined form coherence thresholds were elevated in 90% of children with deprivation amblyopia when viewing with the amblyopic eye and in 40% when viewing with the fellow eye. The deficit was similar in children with a cataract that had been visually significant at birth (congenital) and in children for whom the cataract appeared later in infancy or childhood (developmental). Poorer motion-defined form perception in amblyopic eyes was associated with poorer visual acuity, poorer binocular function, greater interocular suppression, and the presence of nystagmus. Fellow-eye deficits were not associated with any of these factors, but a temporo-nasal asymmetry for global motion perception in favor of nasalward motion suggested a general disruption in motion perception. Conclusions: Deficits in motion-defined form perception are common in children with deprivation amblyopia and may reflect a problem in motion processing that relies on binocular mechanisms.


Assuntos
Ambliopia , Catarata , Percepção de Forma , Percepção de Movimento , Recém-Nascido , Criança , Humanos , Olho
3.
J Vis ; 24(4): 23, 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38662346

RESUMO

This paper reviews projection models and their perception in realistic pictures, and proposes hypotheses for three-dimensional (3D) shape and space perception in pictures. In these hypotheses, eye fixations, and foveal vision play a central role. Many past theories and experimental studies focus solely on linear perspective. Yet, these theories fail to explain many important perceptual phenomena, including the effectiveness of nonlinear projections. Indeed, few classical paintings strictly obey linear perspective, nor do the best distortion-avoidance techniques for wide-angle computational photography. The hypotheses here employ a two-stage model for 3D human vision. When viewing a picture, the first stage perceives 3D shape for the current gaze. Each fixation has its own perspective projection, but, owing to the nature of foveal and peripheral vision, shape information is obtained primarily for a small region of the picture around the fixation. As a viewer moves their eyes, the second stage continually integrates some of the per-gaze information into an overall interpretation of a picture. The interpretation need not be geometrically stable or consistent over time. It is argued that this framework could explain many disparate pictorial phenomena, including different projection styles throughout art history and computational photography, while being consistent with the constraints of human 3D vision. The paper reviews open questions and suggests new studies to explore these hypotheses.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fóvea Central/fisiologia
4.
Vision Res ; 219: 108394, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579407

RESUMO

Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Esquizofrenia , Humanos , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Feminino , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estudos Retrospectivos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(6): 605-625, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573695

RESUMO

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Ilusões Ópticas , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia
6.
Jpn J Ophthalmol ; 68(3): 183-191, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38598144

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To assess the impact of glaucoma on perceiving three-dimensional (3D) shapes based on monocular depth cues. STUDY DESIGN: Clinical observational study. METHODS: Twenty glaucoma patients, subjected to binocular visual-field sensitivity (binocular-VFS) tests using a Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer, and 20 age-matched healthy volunteers, underwent two tasks: identifying the nearest vertex of a 3D shape using monocular shading (3D-SfS), texture (3D-SfT), or motion (3D-SfM) cues, and distinguishing elementary one-dimensional (1D) features of these cues. The association of the visual-field index (VFI) of binocular-VFS with 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients was also examined. RESULTS: Glaucoma patients demonstrated reduced accuracy in distinguishing 1D luminance brightness and a larger "error-in-depth" between the perceived and actual depths for 3D-SfM and 3D-SfS compared to healthy volunteers. Six glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS exhibited a similar error-in-depth to the other fourteen glaucoma patients; they had a larger error-in-depth for 3D-SfM compared to healthy volunteers. No correlation between the error-in-depth values and the VFI values of binocular-VFS was observed. CONCLUSIONS: The 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients varies based on the depth cue's characteristics. Impaired 1D discrimination and larger thresholds for 3D-SfM in glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS indicate more pronounced perceptual deficits of lower-level elementary features for 3D-SfS and higher-level visual processing of 3D shapes for 3D-SfM. The effects of the location and degree of binocular visual-field defects on 3D shape perception remain to be elucidated. Our research provides insights into the 3D shape extraction mechanism in glaucoma.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção de Profundidade , Glaucoma , Visão Binocular , Visão Monocular , Campos Visuais , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso , Glaucoma/fisiopatologia , Glaucoma/diagnóstico , Visão Monocular/fisiologia , Testes de Campo Visual , Pressão Intraocular/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Adulto
7.
Vision Res ; 220: 108400, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38603923

RESUMO

It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse's aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Percepção de Movimento , Estimulação Luminosa , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Julgamento/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Masculino , Feminino , Psicofísica , Adulto Jovem , Análise de Variância , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia
8.
Curr Biol ; 34(5): R195-R197, 2024 Mar 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38471446

RESUMO

The representation of visual shape is a critical component of our perception of the objects around us. A new study exploited shape aftereffects to reveal the high-dimensional space of geometric features our brains use to represent shape.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Percepção Visual , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Visão Ocular , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos
9.
Nature ; 627(8005): 821-829, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38448584

RESUMO

Animals in the natural world constantly encounter geometrically complex landscapes. Successful navigation requires that they understand geometric features of these landscapes, including boundaries, landmarks, corners and curved areas, all of which collectively define the geometry of the environment1-12. Crucial to the reconstruction of the geometric layout of natural environments are concave and convex features, such as corners and protrusions. However, the neural substrates that could underlie the perception of concavity and convexity in the environment remain elusive. Here we show that the dorsal subiculum contains neurons that encode corners across environmental geometries in an allocentric reference frame. Using longitudinal calcium imaging in freely behaving mice, we find that corner cells tune their activity to reflect the geometric properties of corners, including corner angles, wall height and the degree of wall intersection. A separate population of subicular neurons encode convex corners of both larger environments and discrete objects. Both corner cells are non-overlapping with the population of subicular neurons that encode environmental boundaries. Furthermore, corner cells that encode concave or convex corners generalize their activity such that they respond, respectively, to concave or convex curvatures within an environment. Together, our findings suggest that the subiculum contains the geometric information needed to reconstruct the shape and layout of naturalistic spatial environments.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Percepção de Forma , Hipocampo , Neurônios , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Cálcio/análise , Cálcio/metabolismo , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Hipocampo/citologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Neurônios/metabolismo , Neurônios/fisiologia , Propriedades de Superfície
10.
eNeuro ; 11(3)2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38423791

RESUMO

The cortical visual area, V4, has been considered to code contours that contribute to the intermediate-level representation of objects. The neural responses to the complex contour features intrinsic to natural contours are expected to clarify the essence of the representation. To approach the cortical coding of natural contours, we investigated the simultaneous coding of multiple contour features in monkey (Macaca fuscata) V4 neurons and their population-level representation. A substantial number of neurons showed significant tuning for two or more features such as curvature and closure, indicating that a substantial number of V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features. A large portion of the neurons responded vigorously to acutely curved contours that surrounded the center of classical receptive field, suggesting that V4 neurons tend to code prominent features of object contours. The analysis of mutual information (MI) between the neural responses and each contour feature showed that most neurons exhibited similar magnitudes for each type of MI, indicating that many neurons showing the responses depended on multiple contour features. We next examined the population-level representation by using multidimensional scaling analysis. The neural preferences to the multiple contour features and that to natural stimuli compared with silhouette stimuli increased along with the primary and secondary axes, respectively, indicating the contribution of the multiple contour features and surface textures in the population responses. Our analyses suggested that V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features in natural images and represent contour and surface properties in population.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Córtex Visual , Animais , Macaca mulatta , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa
11.
Nat Hum Behav ; 8(2): 320-335, 2024 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37996497

RESUMO

Many surface cues support three-dimensional shape perception, but humans can sometimes still see shape when these features are missing-such as when an object is covered with a draped cloth. Here we propose a framework for three-dimensional shape perception that explains perception in both typical and atypical cases as analysis-by-synthesis, or inference in a generative model of image formation. The model integrates intuitive physics to explain how shape can be inferred from the deformations it causes to other objects, as in cloth draping. Behavioural and computational studies comparing this account with several alternatives show that it best matches human observers (total n = 174) in both accuracy and response times, and is the only model that correlates significantly with human performance on difficult discriminations. We suggest that bottom-up deep neural network models are not fully adequate accounts of human shape perception, and point to how machine vision systems might achieve more human-like robustness.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Humanos , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Redes Neurais de Computação , Sinais (Psicologia)
12.
Perception ; 53(2): 110-124, 2024 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37915210

RESUMO

The watercolor illusion (WCI) occurs when an achromatic region is surrounded by an outer contour and inner chromatic fringe, resulting in an apparent pale tint of the same hue as the fringe. The WCI both fills in and spreads out, with the previous literature suggesting it always spreads out in the absence of an enclosing border. We examined how global stimulus configuration affects this illusion by dissecting various WCI-inducing stimuli into parts. Specifically, would color spread out of the unenclosed ends of the disconnected parts? Participants provided WCI illusion magnitude ratings and shading data indicating perceived locations of color spreading for a variety of stimulus configurations. Instead of the WCI spreading modally into the spaces between the disconnected parts, we found a global reorganization of the stimuli occurred. The dissected WCI stimuli were perceived as either amodally completed behind a white illusory surface perceptually different than the physically identical background or, as empty space between separate objects depending in part on the distance between dissected parts. This study demonstrates the WCI does not always spread outside of unenclosed borders when the global interpretation interferes with spreading. These findings highlight the importance of global configuration and perceptual organization in the WCI.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Ilusões , Ilusões Ópticas , Humanos , Percepção de Cores , Estimulação Luminosa
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(1): 221-236, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37935897

RESUMO

Dynamic occlusion, such as the accretion and deletion of texture near a boundary, is a major factor in determining relative depth of surfaces. However, the shape of the contour bounding the dynamic texture can significantly influence what kind of 3D shape, and what relative depth, are conveyed by the optic flow. This can lead to percepts that are inconsistent with traditional accounts of shape and depth from motion, where accreting/deleting texture can indicate the figural region, and/or 3D rotation can be perceived despite the constant speed of the optic flow. This suggests that the speed profile of the dynamic texture and the shape of its bounding contours combine to determine relative depth in a way that is not explained by existing models. Here, we investigated how traditional structure-from-motion principles and contour geometry interact to determine the relative-depth interpretation of dynamic textures. We manipulated the consistency of the dynamic texture with rotational or translational motion by varying the speed profile of the texture. In Experiment 1, we used a multi-region figure-ground display consisting of regions with dots moving horizontally in opposite directions in adjacent regions. In Experiment 2, we used stimuli including two regions separated by a common border, with dot textures moving horizontally in opposite directions. Both contour geometry (convexity) and the speed profile of the dynamic dot texture influenced relative-depth judgments, but contour geometry was the stronger factor. The results underscore the importance of contour geometry, which most current models disregard, in determining depth from motion.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Percepção de Movimento , Fluxo Óptico , Humanos , Rotação , Percepção de Profundidade
14.
PLoS One ; 18(12): e0295527, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38096196

RESUMO

In this study, we propose a restoration algorithm for distorted objects using a curvature-driven flow. First, we capture the convex-hull shaped contour of the distorted object using the mean curvature flow. With the supplemented mass on the captured feature, we evolve the constraint mean curvature flow to a steady state, preserving the non-distorted region. With respect to the mass, we select a restorative shape by considering the square of the curvature derivative. The Allen-Cahn and Cahn-Hilliard equations are applied to the generated restored image from the implicit curvature motions represented by the order parameter. We impose the Dirichlet boundary condition for the order parameter and the Neumann boundary for the chemical potential to fix the feature and to inherit the mass conservation, respectively. We provided examples of the restoration of half-circle and parentheses-shaped objects to reconstruct a circle shape.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Percepção de Forma , Movimento (Física)
15.
Neural Comput ; 36(1): 33-74, 2023 Dec 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38052088

RESUMO

Under difficult viewing conditions, the brain's visual system uses a variety of recurrent modulatory mechanisms to augment feedforward processing. One resulting phenomenon is contour integration, which occurs in the primary visual (V1) cortex and strengthens neural responses to edges if they belong to a larger smooth contour. Computational models have contributed to an understanding of the circuit mechanisms of contour integration, but less is known about its role in visual perception. To address this gap, we embedded a biologically grounded model of contour integration in a task-driven artificial neural network and trained it using a gradient-descent variant. We used this model to explore how brain-like contour integration may be optimized for high-level visual objectives as well as its potential roles in perception. When the model was trained to detect contours in a background of random edges, a task commonly used to examine contour integration in the brain, it closely mirrored the brain in terms of behavior, neural responses, and lateral connection patterns. When trained on natural images, the model enhanced weaker contours and distinguished whether two points lay on the same versus different contours. The model learned robust features that generalized well to out-of-training-distribution stimuli. Surprisingly, and in contrast with the synthetic task, a parameter-matched control network without recurrence performed the same as or better than the model on the natural-image tasks. Thus, a contour integration mechanism is not essential to perform these more naturalistic contour-related tasks. Finally, the best performance in all tasks was achieved by a modified contour integration model that did not distinguish between excitatory and inhibitory neurons.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Córtex Visual , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Aprendizagem
16.
J Vis ; 23(14): 4, 2023 Dec 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38091030

RESUMO

Gestalten in visual perception are defined by emergent properties of the whole, which cannot be predicted from the sum of its parts; rather, they arise by virtue of inherent principles, the Laws of Seeing. This review attempts to assign neurophysiological correlates to select emergent properties in motion and contour perception and proposes parallels to the processing of local versus global attributes by classical versus contextual receptive fields. The aim is to identify Gestalt neurons in the visual system to account for the Laws of Seeing in causal terms and to explain "Why do things look as they do" (Koffka, 1935, p. 76).


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Percepção de Movimento , Humanos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia
17.
Sci Adv ; 9(46): eadj3906, 2023 11 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37967184

RESUMO

Visual illusions provide valuable insights into the brain's interpretation of the world given sensory inputs. However, the precise manner in which brain activity translates into illusory experiences remains largely unknown. Here, we leverage a brain decoding technique combined with deep neural network (DNN) representations to reconstruct illusory percepts as images from brain activity. The reconstruction model was trained on natural images to establish a link between brain activity and perceptual features and then tested on two types of illusions: illusory lines and neon color spreading. Reconstructions revealed lines and colors consistent with illusory experiences, which varied across the source visual cortical areas. This framework offers a way to materialize subjective experiences, shedding light on the brain's internal representations of the world.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Ilusões , Córtex Visual , Humanos , Encéfalo , Redes Neurais de Computação , Percepção Visual
18.
Curr Biol ; 33(20): R1042-R1044, 2023 10 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37875075

RESUMO

The human visual system is tasked with the problem of extracting information about the world from images that contain a conflated mixture of environmental sources and optical artifacts generated by the focal properties of our eyes. In most contexts, our brains manage to distinguish these sources, but this is not always the case. Recent work showed that shading gradients generated by smooth three-dimensional (3D) surfaces can elicit strong illusory percepts of optical defocus1,2 - the perception of illusory blur is only eliminated when the surface appears attached to self-occluding contours3, surface discontinuities1, or sharp specular reflections1,2, which all generate sharp ('high spatial frequency') image structure. This suggests that it should also be possible to eliminate the illusory blur elicited by shaded surfaces by altering the surface geometry to include small-scale surface relief, which would also generate high-frequency image structure. We report the surprising result here that this manipulation fails to eliminate the perception of blur; the fine texture fails to perceptually 'bind' to the low-frequency image structure when there is a sufficient gap between the spatial scales of the fine and coarse surface structure. These findings suggest that discontinuous 'gaps' in the spatial scale of textures are a segmentation cue the visual system uses to extract multiple causes of image structure.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Ilusões , Humanos , Olho , Encéfalo
19.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 12748, 2023 08 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37550419

RESUMO

It has been known for more than 160 years that highly occluded objects that would normally be visually unrecognizable can be successfully identified when they move. This anorthoscopic perception relies on the visual system's ability to integrate information over time to complete the perception of an entire object's shape. In this experiment, 16 younger and older adults (mean ages were 20.5 and 74.6 years, respectively) were familiarized with the (unoccluded) shapes of five naturally-shaped objects (bell peppers, Capsicum annuum) until they could be easily identified (i.e., with accuracies of at least 90 percent correct). All observers then viewed the stimulus objects anorthoscopically as they moved behind narrow slits; only small object fragments could be seen at any given time, because the objects were almost totally occluded from view. Even though the object identification performance for all observers was equivalent when whole object shapes were visible, a large age-related deficit in object identification emerged during anorthoscopic viewing such that the younger adults' identification performance was 45.4 percent higher than that of the older adults. This first ever study of aging and anorthoscopic perception demonstrates that there is an age-related deficit in performing the temporal integration needed for successful object recognition.


Assuntos
Percepção Visual , Humanos , Adulto Jovem , Idoso , Capsicum , Percepção de Forma
20.
Transgenic Res;32(5):487-496,2023
em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-937540410

RESUMO

β1,3-galactose is the component of outer-chain elongation of complex N-glycans that, together with α1,4-fucose, forms Lewis a structures in plants. Previous studies have revealed that N-glycan maturation is mediated by sequential attachment of β1,3-galactose and α1,4-fucose by individual β1,3-galactosyltransferase (GalT) and α1,4-fucosyltransferase (1,4-FucT), respectively. Although GalT from several species has been studied, little information about GalT from rice is available. I therefore characterized three GalT candidate genes on different chromosomes in Oryza sativa. Seeds of rice lines that had T-DNA insertions in regions corresponding to individual putative GalT genes were obtained from a Rice Functional Genomic Express Database and plants grown until maturity. Homozygotes were selected from the next generation by genotyping PCR, and used for callus induction. Callus extracts of two independent T-DNA mutant rice which have T-DNA insertions at the same gene on chromosome 6 but in different exons showed highly reduced band intensity on a western blots using an anti-Lewis a antibody. Cell extracts and cultured media from suspension culture of the one of these mutant rice were further analysed by N-glycan profiling using matrix-associated laser desorption/ionization-time of flight mass spectrometry (MALDI-TOF). Identified N-glycan species containing β1,3-galactose from both cell extracts and cultured media of knock-out mutant were less than 0.5% of total N-glycans while that of WT cells were 9.8% and 49.1%, respectively. This suggests that GalT located on rice chromosome 6 plays a major role in N-glycan galactosylation, and mutations within it lead to blockage of Lewis a epitope formation.


Assuntos
Radiação não IonizanteNucleotídeos de Adenina , 5643 , Doenças dos Bovinos , Ácido FormiminoglutâmicoNucleotídeos de Adenina , 6801 , Radiação não IonizanteNucleotídeos de Adenina , Cromatografia em Agarose , Percepção de Forma , 11134Nucleotídeos de Adenina
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...