Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 41
Filtrar
2.
Univ. psychol ; 17(3): 109-119, jul.-set. 2018. tab, graf
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS, COLNAL | ID: biblio-979521

RESUMO

Resumen El objetivo de este estudio, consistió en evaluar la familiaridad subjetiva (FS) de 827 palabras en 187 niños de segundo grado de educación primaria de la ciudad de Medellín. Las palabras, fueron extraídas de diferentes textos escolares y se ajustaron a tres parámetros psicolingüísticos: consistencia ortográfica, estructura silábica y longitud. En la prueba realizada, los niños debían indicar si conocían nada, poco, mucho o muchísimo cada una de las palabras seleccionadas. De acuerdo con esto, se asignó un valor numérico a cada palabra. Un análisis estadístico de las medianas de las palabras, permitió seleccionar 405 palabras con FS alta y 422 palabras con FS baja. Los resultados, muestran que los parámetros psicolingüísticos propuestos se relacionan con la FS.


Abstract The aim of this study was to assess the subjective familiarity of 827 words in 187 second-grade children from the city of Medellín. The words were taken from textbooks and adjusted to three psycholinguistic parameters: orthographic consistency, syllabic structure and length. In these tests, the children had to indicate whether they knew each of the words and according to their responses, a numerical value was assigned. A statistical analysis of the medians allowed the selection of 405 words of high subjective familiarity y 422 words of low subjective familiarity. The results show that the proposed psycholinguistic parameters relate to subjective familiarity.


Assuntos
Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia
3.
Stat Methods Med Res ; 26(6): 2708-2725, 2017 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26400088

RESUMO

In multiple fields of study, time series measured at high frequencies are used to estimate population curves that describe the temporal evolution of some characteristic of interest. These curves are typically nonlinear, and the deviations of each series from the corresponding curve are highly autocorrelated. In this scenario, we propose a procedure to compare the response curves for different groups at specific points in time. The method involves fitting the curves, performing potentially hundreds of serially correlated tests, and appropriately adjusting the overall alpha level of the tests. Our motivating application comes from psycholinguistics and the visual world paradigm. We describe how the proposed technique can be adapted to compare fixation curves within subjects as well as between groups. Our results lead to conclusions beyond the scope of previous analyses.


Assuntos
Dinâmica não Linear , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Estimulação Acústica , Algoritmos , Bioestatística/métodos , Implantes Cocleares , Simulação por Computador , Humanos , Idioma , Modelos Logísticos , Modelos Estatísticos , Distribuição Normal , Fatores de Tempo
4.
Psicológica (Valencia, Ed. impr.) ; 38(1): 111-131, 2017. tab, ilus, graf
Artigo em Inglês | IBECS | ID: ibc-161215

RESUMO

With the increasing refinement of language processing models and the new discoveries about which variables can modulate these processes, stimuli selection for experiments with a factorial design is becoming a tough task. Selecting sets of words that differ in one variable, while matching these same words into dozens of other confounding variables is time consuming and error prone. To assist experimenters in this thankless task, we present a simple method to perform it with little effort. The method is based on Kmeans clustering as a way to detect small and tight clusters of words that match in the desired variables. We have formalized the procedure into an algorithmic format, that is, a series of easy-to-follow steps. In addition, we also provide an SPSS syntax that helps in choosing the correct size of the clustering. After reviewing the theory, we present a worked example that will guide the reader through the complete procedure. The dataset of the worked example is available as a supplementary material to this paper (AU)


Con el creciente refinamiento de los modelos de procesamiento del lenguaje y los nuevos hallazgos sobre qué variables pueden modular dichos procesos, la selección de palabras para experimentos de diseño factorial se está convirtiendo en una tarea cada vez más ardua. Seleccionar conjuntos de palabras que difieren en una variable pero que están igualadas en una decena de posibles variables extrañas, lleva mucho tiempo y está sujeto a errores. Para ayudar a los experimentadores en esta desagradecida tarea, presentamos un método sencillo que permite realizarla con poco esfuerzo. El método se basa en el agrupamiento de Kmedias para identificar conjuntos pequeños y compactos de palabras igualadas en las variables deseadas. El procedimiento ha sido formalizado en un algoritmo, esto es, una serie de pasos concretos y sencillos de seguir. Además, también aportamos la sintaxis en SPSS para ayudar en la selección del número adecuado de agrupaciones. Tras una revisión de la teoría, presentamos un ejemplo práctico que guiará al lector a través del procedimiento completo. El conjunto de datos del ejemplo se encuentra disponible como material complementario a este artículo (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Algoritmos , Análise Fatorial , Testes de Associação de Palavras/estatística & dados numéricos , Idioma , Psicologia Experimental/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos
5.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(3): 963-72, 2016 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26170053

RESUMO

Lexical frequency is one of the strongest predictors of word processing time. The frequencies are often calculated from book-based corpora, or more recently from subtitle-based corpora. We present new frequencies based on Twitter, blog posts, or newspapers for 66 languages. We show that these frequencies predict lexical decision reaction times similar to the already existing frequencies, or even better than them. These new frequencies are freely available and may be downloaded from http://worldlex.lexique.org .


Assuntos
Idioma , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Mídias Sociais/estatística & dados numéricos , Blogging , Humanos
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 68(8): 1457-68, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25975773

RESUMO

This paper introduces and summarizes the special issue on megastudies, crowdsourcing, and large datasets in psycholinguistics. We provide a brief historical overview and show how the papers in this issue have extended the field by compiling new databases and making important theoretical contributions. In addition, we discuss several studies that use text corpora to build distributional semantic models to tackle various interesting problems in psycholinguistics. Finally, as is the case across the papers, we highlight some methodological issues that are brought forth via the analyses of such datasets.


Assuntos
Crowdsourcing , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/tendências , Crowdsourcing/estatística & dados numéricos , Crowdsourcing/tendências , História do Século XX , História do Século XXI , Humanos , Psicolinguística/história , Semântica
7.
Behav Res Methods ; 46(1): 284-306, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23784009

RESUMO

The morphological constituents of English compounds (e.g., "butter" and "fly" for "butterfly") and two-character Chinese compounds may differ in meaning from the whole word. Subjective differences and ambiguity of transparency make judgments difficult, and a computational alternative based on a general model might be a way to average across subjective differences. In the present study, we propose two approaches based on latent semantic analysis (Landauer & Dumais in Psychological Review 104:211-240, 1997): Model 1 compares the semantic similarity between a compound word and each of its constituents, and Model 2 derives the dominant meaning of a constituent from a clustering analysis of morphological family members (e.g., "butterfingers" or "buttermilk" for "butter"). The proposed models successfully predicted participants' transparency ratings, and we recommend that experimenters use Model 1 for English compounds and Model 2 for Chinese compounds, on the basis of differences in raters' morphological processing in the different writing systems. The dominance of lexical meaning, semantic transparency, and the average similarity between all pairs within a morphological family are provided, and practical applications for future studies are discussed.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Idioma , Modelos Psicológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Psicolinguística/métodos , Semântica , Adulto , Área Sob a Curva , Povo Asiático , Feminino , Humanos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Curva ROC , Vocabulário
8.
Rev. logop. foniatr. audiol. (Ed. impr.) ; 31(2): 57-64, abr.-jun. 2011.
Artigo em Espanhol | IBECS | ID: ibc-89806

RESUMO

Una idea muy extendida en la psicología del lenguaje considera que las categorías formales y funcionales utilizadas en lingüística estructural para describir los hechos de lengua tienen realidad psicológica para la organización de los enunciados. A continuación demostramos que esta creencia no es sostenible. Una orientación teórica alternativa prevé una traducción directa de las relaciones de significado, proporcionadas por una matriz semántica que interviene dentro de un marco pragmático, en secuencias léxicas de superficie; y los registros productivos se constituyen a través de analogías sustitutivas aprendidas de forma implícita. Esta afirmación, mucho más cerca de la opinión de los partidarios de las neurociencias cognitivas, tiene importantes implicaciones para la terapia de rehabilitación en logopedia de la morfosintaxis. © 2010 AELFA. Publicado por Elsevier España, S.L. Todos los derechos reservados (AU)


A widely held idea in the psychology of language is that the formal and functional categories used in structural linguistics to describe language phenomena have a psychological reality for the organization of utterances. In the present article, we demonstrate that this belief is untenable. An alternative theoretical approach envisages direct translation of meaning relations, provided by a semantic matrix that intervenes within a pragmatic framework, in superficial lexical sequences; productive registers are created through implicitly learned substitution analogies. This affirmation, which is closer to the viewpoint of the cognitive neurosciences, has important implications for the speech therapy of morphosyntactic difficulties (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Criança , Transtornos da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Semântica , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental/métodos , Ciência Cognitiva/métodos , Psicolinguística/métodos , Audiologia/métodos , Audiologia/tendências , Fonoaudiologia/tendências , Transtornos da Linguagem/etiologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/terapia , Neurociências/métodos , Neurociências/tendências , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/tendências , Conhecimento
9.
Behav Res Methods ; 43(2): 449-58, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21416306

RESUMO

Frequency of occurrence is an important attribute of lexical units, and one that is widely used in psychological research and theorization. Although printed frequency norms have long been available for Spanish, and subtitle-based norms have more recently been published, oral frequency norms have not been systematically compiled for a representative set of words. In this study, a corpus of over three million units, representing present-day use of the language in Spain, was used to derive a frequency count of spoken words. The corpus consisted of 913 separate documents that contained transcriptions of oral recordings obtained in a wide variety of situations, mostly radio and television programs. The resulting database, containing absolute and relative frequency values for 67,979 orally produced words, is presented. Validity analyses showed significant correlations of oral frequency with other frequency measures and suggest that oral frequency can predict some types of lexical processing with the same or higher levels of precision, when contrasted with text- or subtitle-based frequencies. In conclusion, we discuss ways in which these oral frequency norms can be put to use. The norms can be downloaded from www.springerlink.com.


Assuntos
Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Fala , Vocabulário , Humanos , Idioma
10.
Behav Res Methods ; 43(1): 155-67, 2011 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21287108

RESUMO

Amazon's Mechanical Turk (AMT) is a Web application that provides instant access to thousands of potential participants for survey-based psychology experiments, such as the acceptability judgment task used extensively in syntactic theory. Because AMT is a Web-based system, syntacticians may worry that the move out of the experimenter-controlled environment of the laboratory and onto the user-controlled environment of AMT could adversely affect the quality of the judgment data collected. This article reports a quantitative comparison of two identical acceptability judgment experiments, each with 176 participants (352 total): one conducted in the laboratory, and one conducted on AMT. Crucial indicators of data quality--such as participant rejection rates, statistical power, and the shape of the distributions of the judgments for each sentence type--are compared between the two samples. The results suggest that aside from slightly higher participant rejection rates, AMT data are almost indistinguishable from laboratory data.


Assuntos
Coleta de Dados , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Custos e Análise de Custo , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Inglaterra , Feminino , Humanos , Ilusões/psicologia , Internet , Julgamento , Idioma , Linguística/métodos , Linguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Software , Adulto Jovem
11.
Behav Res Methods ; 43(1): 124-35, 2011 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21287124

RESUMO

This study provides implicit verb causality norms for a corpus of 305 English verbs. A web-based sentence completion study was conducted, with 96 respondents completing fragments such as "John liked Mary because..." The resulting bias scores are provided as supplementary material in the Psychonomic Society Archive, where we also present lexical and semantic verb features, such as the frequency, semantic class and emotional valence. Our results replicate those of previous studies with much smaller numbers of verbs and respondents. Novel effects of gender and its interaction with verb valence illustrate the type of issues that can be investigated using stable norms for a large number of verbs. The corpus will facilitate future studies in a range of areas, including psycholinguistics and social psychology.


Assuntos
Idioma , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/normas , Adulto , Emoções , Inglaterra , Feminino , Humanos , Internet , Masculino , Psicolinguística/classificação , Desempenho Psicomotor , Valores de Referência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Caracteres Sexuais
12.
Behav Res Methods ; 43(1): 110-23, 2011 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21287129

RESUMO

The present study reports descriptive normative measures for 245 Italian verbal idiomatic expressions. For each of the idiomatic expressions the following variables are reported: Length, Knowledge, Familiarity, Age of Acquisition, Predictability, Syntactic flexibility, Literality and Compositionality. Syntactic flexibility was assessed using five syntactic operations: adverb insertion, adjective insertion, left dislocation, passive and movement. The psycholinguistic relevance of each dimension, their measures and the correlations among them are provided and discussed. The databases are freely available for down-loading from the Psychonomic Society Web archive at www.psychonomic.org/archive/.


Assuntos
Idioma , Psicolinguística/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Coleta de Dados , Bases de Dados Factuais , Feminino , Humanos , Itália , Conhecimento , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Valores de Referência , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
13.
Psicológica (Valencia, Ed. impr.) ; 32(2): 171-206, 2011.
Artigo em Espanhol | IBECS | ID: ibc-89485

RESUMO

El presente estudio se planteó con el objetivo de calcular el índice psicolingüístico de concreción de una serie de palabras que resultan de interés en la investigación psicológica. El corpus analizado constó de 730 palabras, la mayoría de las cuales son ejemplares frecuentes de categorías naturales de acuerdo con normas de producción categorial en castellano. El resto, que fueron añadidas con fines metodológicos, se tomaron de una de las bases de datos de índices psicolingüísticos más frecuentemente utilizadas en castellano. Esta inclusión nos permitió, además, actualizar los valores de concreción de estas palabras y poner a prueba la estabilidad de dicho índice. Los resultados apoyan la estabilidad del mismo ante cambios generacionales y cambios en el tamaño muestral. Los datos obtenidos permiten un empleo más sofisticado de los estímulos verbales a los profesionales que trabajan en el campo de la investigación, la neuropsicología clínica y la educación. Finalmente, el estudio expone las principales limitaciones asociadas a los estudios normativos de obtención de índices psicolingüísticos y propone estrategias para su afrontamiento(AU)


The present study was aimed at compiling the concreteness values for a pool of words of particular interest to psychological research. The analyzed set included 730 words, most of which were common exemplars of natural categories according to Spanish-language norms. The rest of the words, which were added for methodological reasons, were selected from one of the most frequently used databases of psycholinguistic indexes in Spanish. This inclusion gave us the opportunity to update the concreteness values of these words as well as testing the stability of this index. The results support the stability of this index despite generational changes and changes in the sample size. The data obtained in this study enable researchers, clinicians and other professionals to make a more sophisticated usage of the verbal stimuli. Finally, this paper identifies the main limitations associated to normative studies for obtaining psycholinguistic indexes and suggests strategies for addressing these limitations(AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Psicolinguística/educação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Pesquisa/métodos , Idioma , Cognição/fisiologia , Dislexia/psicologia , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/normas , Psicolinguística/tendências , Inquéritos e Questionários
14.
Psicológica (Valencia, Ed. impr.) ; 30(2): 323-342, jul.-dic. 2009.
Artigo em Espanhol | IBECS | ID: ibc-73755

RESUMO

El efecto de generación ha demostrado ser una técnica útil para mejorar el aprendizaje y el recuerdo de muy diversos tipos de materiales. Sin embargo no se ha encontrado este efecto en materiales sin significado, como las pseudopalabras, por lo que es lógico pensar que la familiaridad de los estímulos (su frecuencia y edad de adquisición) podría influir en la eficacia de esta técnica. Para comprobar esta hipótesis se llevaron a cabo dos experimentos en los que se manipulaban la frecuencia y la edad de adquisición. La potencia del efecto de generación impidió que se manifestaran los efectos de estas dos variables en la tarea de recuerdo libre, pero ambas influyeron en la tarea de reconocimiento. Así, se obtuvo "efecto espejo" tanto de frecuencia como de edad de adquisición, de manera que los estímulos menos familiares fueron mejor reconocidos que los más familiares (AU)


The generation effect has demonstrated to be an effective technique to improve learning and memory of several types of materials. However, this effect has not been found when meaningless materials are used, such as pseudo words. That is why it is logical to think that the familiarity of the stimuli (their frequency and age of acquisition) could influence the efficacy of this technique. To test this hypothesis two experiments, in which the frequency and the age of acquisition were manipulated, were carried out. The power of the generation effect did not allow the effects of these two variables to become evident in the recall task, but both influenced there cognition task. A "mirror effect" of frequency and age of acquisition was obtained, as the participants recognized the unfamiliar better than the familiar stimuli (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Efeito de Coortes , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Estudantes/psicologia , Estudantes/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/tendências , Inquéritos e Questionários , Análise de Variância
15.
Behav Res Methods ; 41(4): 1009-17, 2009 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19897809

RESUMO

The LEXIN database offers psycholinguistic indexes of the 13,184 different words (types) computed from 178,839 occurrences of these words (tokens) contained in a corpus of 134 beginning readers widely used in Spain. This database provides four statistical indicators: F (overall word frequency), D (index of dispersion across selected readers), U (estimated frequency per million words), and SFI (standard frequency index). It also gives information about the number of letters, syntactic category, and syllabic structure of the words included. To facilitate comparisons, LEXIN provides data from LEXESP's (Sebastián-Gallés, Martí, Cuetos, & Carreiras, 2000), Alameda and Cuetos's (1995), and Martínez and García's (2004) Spanish adult psycholinguistic frequency databases. Access to the LEXIN database is facilitated by a computer program. The LEXIN program allows for the creation of word lists by letting the user specify searching criteria. LEXIN can be useful for researchers in cognitive psychology, particularly in the areas of psycholinguistics and education.


Assuntos
Bases de Dados Factuais , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Computadores , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Fonética , Semântica , Software , Espanha , Vocabulário
16.
Behav Res Methods ; 41(2): 452-71, 2009 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19363186

RESUMO

The internal validity of several types of experiments in experimental psychology and neuroscience depends in part on the possibility of controlling or manipulating critical lexical variables such as word frequency of occurrence. Two ways of estimating this variable are (1) objective frequency counts and (2) subjective ratings of word frequency. Each method produces estimates that generally agree (i.e., they are highly correlated) but that disagree substantially concerning the relative frequency of a number of words. To investigate this issue more closely, the global and local agreement of subjective frequency estimates was examined in detail for a pool of 6,202 words drawn from the OMNILEX database of French words (Desrochers, 2006; www.omnilex.uottawa.ca). The results indicated that objective and subjective frequencies are strongly correlated, subjective frequencies share a significant amount of bias variance with other lexical characteristics (e.g., imageability), and the codeterminants of subjective frequency are in an antagonistic relationship with one another. The implications of these results for the selection of lexical stimuli are discussed, and multiple variables to aid in item selection are reported. Supplemental materials for this study may be downloaded from brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Assuntos
Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Bases de Dados Factuais , Humanos , Modelos Estatísticos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
17.
Psychol Assess ; 21(1): 79-88, 2009 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19290768

RESUMO

Psychological interventions provide linguistic data that are particularly useful for testing mechanisms of action and improving intervention methodologies. For this study, emotional expression in an Internet-based intervention for women with breast cancer (n = 63) was analyzed via rater coding and 2 computerized coding methods (Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count [LIWC] and Psychiatric Content Analysis and Diagnosis [PCAD]). Although the computerized coding methods captured most of the emotion identified by raters (LIWC sensitivity = .88; PCAD sensitivity = .83), both over-identified emotional expression (LIWC positive predictive value = .31; PCAD positive predictive value = .19). Correlational analyses suggested better convergent and discriminant validity for LIWC. The results highlight previously unrecognized deficiencies in commonly used computerized content-analysis programs and suggest potential modifications to both programs that could improve overall accuracy of automated identification of emotional expression. Although the authors recognize these limitations, they conclude that LIWC is superior to PCAD for rapid identification of emotional expression in text. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2009 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/psicologia , Processamento Eletrônico de Dados/métodos , Emoções Manifestas , Adaptação Psicológica , Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/etiologia , Ansiedade/terapia , Neoplasias da Mama/complicações , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/etiologia , Depressão/terapia , Análise Discriminante , Processamento Eletrônico de Dados/estatística & dados numéricos , Feminino , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Qualidade de Vida , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Autorrevelação , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico , Apoio Social , Estresse Psicológico/diagnóstico , Estresse Psicológico/etiologia , Estresse Psicológico/terapia
18.
Law Hum Behav ; 33(3): 258-64, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18633698

RESUMO

Attorneys' language has been found to influence the accuracy of a child's testimony, with defense attorneys asking more complex questions than the prosecution (Zajac & Hayne, J. Exp Psychol Appl 9:187-195, 2003; Zajac et al. Psychiatr Psychol Law, 10:199-209, 2003). These complex questions may be used as a strategy to influence the jury's perceived accuracy of child witnesses. However, we currently do not know whether the complexity of attorney's questions predict the trial outcome. The present study assesses whether the complexity of questions is related to the trial outcome in 46 child sexual abuse court transcripts using an automated linguistic analysis. Based on the complexity of defense attorney's questions, the trial verdict was accurately predicted 82.6% of the time. Contrary to our prediction, more complex questions asked by the defense were associated with convictions, not acquittals.


Assuntos
Abuso Sexual na Infância , Advogados/psicologia , Psicolinguística , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Crime , Feminino , Psiquiatria Legal , Humanos , Função Jurisdicional , Jurisprudência , Idioma , Modelos Logísticos , Los Angeles , Masculino , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos
19.
Psychol Rep ; 102(2): 597-600, 2008 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18567229

RESUMO

Although different in terms of purpose, word-selection procedures, and rating scales, both the ANEW (n = 1034) and DAL (n = 8742) lists, which have 633 words in common, provide normative emotional ratings for English words. This research compared the lists and cross-validated the two main lexical dimensions of affect. Parallel representatives of the two dimensions (Valence and Pleasantness, Arousal and Activation) were correlated across lists (rs = .86, .63). In tune with their separate purposes, the ANEW list, which was designed to describe emotional words, included more rare words, while the DAL, which was designed for natural language applications, included more common ones. The Valence-Activation scatterplot for ANEW was C-shaped and included fewer Arousing words of medium Valence, such as "awake," "debate," and "proves," while the DAL included fewer less common words descriptive of emotion such as "maniac," "corrupt," and "lavish." In view of these differences, list similarities strongly support the generalizability of the two main lexical dimensions of affect.


Assuntos
Dicionários como Assunto , Emoções , Idioma , Psicolinguística , Afeto , Nível de Alerta , Atenção , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Processamento de Linguagem Natural , Fonética , Análise de Componente Principal , Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Semântica
20.
Behav Res Methods ; 40(1): 84-93, 2008 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18411530

RESUMO

Native speakers of languages perceive differences in the acceptability of phrases even when those phrases are both grammatical and novel (previously unseen). We suggest that smoothing, a statistical technique used by natural language processing engineers, provides several candidate mechanisms for investigating this phenomenon. We describe the creation of a large data set of predictions from several smoothing algorithms about the acceptability of unseen grammatical phrases and a novel experimental method for the pairwise comparison of these models. We use this method to compare three smoothing methods and consider the results in light of the differences among the models. We argue that the data support the idea that similarity in this domain is best thought of as a form of asymmetric representational distortion and that the informational basis over which such estimates are made is broad, rather than narrow, as has been previously suggested.


Assuntos
Psicolinguística/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , Algoritmos , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Redes Neurais de Computação , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...