Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
1.
Dyslexia ; 29(3): 255-263, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37169598

RESUMEN

Recent research indicates that awareness of the prosodic information present in spoken language could be an important factor for literacy development, and that adults with developmental dyslexia show impaired awareness of lexical prosodic information, while the phonological representations remain intact. We investigated lexical prosodic representation and awareness in Japanese children with and without developmental dyslexia. Lexical prosodic representation was investigated using a cross-modal fragment priming task, and awareness was examined using a fragment identification task. The task was modified for children by selecting words with higher familiarity and fewer trials. As a result, the same pattern of prosodic priming effects was observed between groups; lexical decision time was faster in the prosodic congruent condition than in the incongruent condition. In addition, accuracy and reaction time did not show group differences in the fragment identification task. Relationship between prosody and literacy development may differ between languages but the sample size were small in both groups. Further investigation with larger sample size is required.

2.
Conscious Cogn ; 106: 103433, 2022 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36343602

RESUMEN

To clarify the feeling of knowing a name but not being able to recall it, known as having a "tip-of-the-tongue" (TOT) experience, we proposed a TOT model consisting of three stages combining a face recognition model and autonomic sympathetic nerve activity. Since TOT increases with age, we compared two age groups: young (N = 27, M = 20.4 ± 1.5 years) and middle-aged (N = 29, M = 58.5 ± 8.0 years). Experiment 1 showed that successfully naming low-frequency common names increased the skin conductance response (SCR) value, and the time to reach the maximum SCR value was longer. Experiment 2 was a naming task for face photographs. The younger group showed higher SCR values during successfully naming, while the middle-aged group showed similar SCR values for successfully naming and experiencing TOT. Both groups had the longest time to reach maximum SCR in TOT. In this study, physiological arousal of TOT was not affected by aging.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Nombres , Persona de Mediana Edad , Humanos , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Anciano , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Lengua/fisiología
3.
Brain Cogn ; 90: 41-9, 2014 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24983819

RESUMEN

We conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study to elucidate the neurocognitive mechanisms of harmful and helpful dishonest decisions. During scanning, the subjects read scenarios concerning events that could occur in real-life situations and were asked to decide whether to tell a lie as though they were experiencing those events. Half of the scenarios consisted of harmful stories in which the dishonest decisions could be regarded as bad lies, and the other half consisted of helpful stories in which the dishonest decisions could be regarded as good lies. In contrast to the control decision-making task, we found that the decision-making tasks that involved honesty or dishonesty recruited a network of brain regions that included the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In the harmful stories, the right temporoparietal junction and the right medial frontal cortex were activated when the subjects made dishonest decisions compared with honest decisions. No region discriminated between the honest and dishonest decisions made in the helpful stories. These preliminary findings suggest that the neural basis of dishonest decisions is modulated by whether the lying serves to harm or help the target.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Decepción , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Intern Med ; 62(22): 3405-3412, 2023 Nov 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37062736

RESUMEN

Cerebellar injuries can cause syntax impairments. Cortical dysfunction due to cerebello-cerebral diaschisis is assumed to play a role in this phenomenon. Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have repeatedly shown the activation of Broca's area in response to syntactic tasks. However, there have been no reports of selective syntax impairment and hypoperfusion restricted to this area after cerebellar injury. We herein report a patient with right cerebellar hemorrhage that led to marked syntax impairment along with severe hypoperfusion confined to the Brodmann area (BA) 45 (anterior part of Broca's area) and BA46.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Lenguaje , Humanos , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Hemorragia Cerebral/complicaciones , Hemorragia Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Frontal/diagnóstico por imagen
5.
Hippocampus ; 22(2): 141-8, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20882553

RESUMEN

Numerous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have reported that the medial temporal lobe (MTL) is activated to a greater extent when subjects encounter novel items as compared with familiar ones. However, it remains unclear whether the novelty signals in the MTL are modulated by the criteria for old/new recognition judgments. In this study, we used fMRI to test our hypothesis that when subjects encounter items similar to previously encountered ones, the novelty signals in the MTL will differ depending on whether the subjects focus on the perceptual features or the semantic aspects of the items. The subjects studied a series of photographs and were later asked to make a recognition judgment of (a) Same items (items identical to those seen during encoding), (b) Similar items (items similar to but not identical to those seen during encoding), and (c) New items (unstudied items) in two types of tasks: Perceptual and Semantic. The subjects judged whether the items were perceptually identical to those seen during encoding in the Perceptual task and whether the items were semantically identical to those seen during encoding in the Semantic task. The left anterior hippocampus was activated when subjects were presented with New items relative to Same items in both tasks. In addition, the hippocampal activity in response to the Similar items was increased only in the Perceptual, but not the Semantic task. Our results indicate that the novelty signals in the hippocampus can be modulated by criteria for old/new recognition judgments.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Hipocampo/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Interpretación de Imagen Asistida por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
6.
Brain Dev ; 44(1): 17-29, 2022 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34481664

RESUMEN

AIMS: To investigate the convergent validity of a new questionnaire, the Developmental Coordination Disorder Checklist (DCDC), we examined the relationship between the DCDC score and Soft Neurological Signs (SNS) which highly correlated with the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC), which is widely applied to assess fine and gross motor skills. METHODS: The patients were 70 elementary school children without intellectual disabilities who were referred to our clinic, examined for SNS, and whose parents completed the DCDC. The DCDC consists of 16 items that focus on three factors: fine motor, gross motor, and catch and throw. We used a previously described method to assess SNS, which included five tasks: (1) standing on one leg with closed eyes, (2) finger opposition test, (3) diadochokinesis, (4) associated movements during diadochokinesis, and (5) motor persistence (laterally fixed gaze). RESULTS: DCDC scores and SNS were strongly and positively correlated, with higher DCDC scores (indicating greater coordination problems) associated with a higher number of positive SNS. CONCLUSIONS: DCDC scores have been proven to be strongly correlated with SNS, and these data suggest that DCDC has good convergent validity.


Asunto(s)
Técnicas de Diagnóstico Neurológico/normas , Trastornos de la Destreza Motora/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Destreza Motora/fisiopatología , Lista de Verificación/normas , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios/normas
7.
Brain Dev ; 43(9): 893-903, 2021 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34052036

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder which occurs in childhood but continues to influence academic and occupational function in adulthood. Recently, a Japanese dyslexia questionnaire and diagnostic procedure was established for primary school children. However, there is currently no procedure for the diagnosis or screening of dyslexia in individuals at or above junior high school age; accordingly, we aimed to develop a questionnaire to screen for reading difficulties in those individuals. METHODS: A questionnaire with various candidate items was developed from two English questionnaires, one Japanese questionnaire, and newly devised items focusing on the Japanese writing system and the most appropriate 28 items were selected. In total, 462 adults and 127 junior high to high school students were enrolled. Of those, 191 participants also took part in reading tests. After the exploratory factor analysis, reliability and validity were evaluated using the above control participants and 12 adolescents with dyslexia. RESULTS: The questionnaire included three factors, i.e., silent reading sub-scale (four items), writing sub-scale (four items), and aloud reading sub-scale (three items). Five were newly devised items focusing on the Japanese writing system. Cronbach's alphas of the three factors were 0.706, 0.638, and 0.568, respectively, and the interclass correlation coefficients (2,1) were 0.743, 0.609, and 0.695, respectively. The silent reading and aloud reading sub-scales were positively correlated with word, non-word, and passage reading time. DISCUSSION: The newly developed questionnaire correlated well with actual reading performance and may be used to screen reading difficulty in Japanese individuals at or above junior high school age.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Lectura , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Escritura , Adulto Joven
8.
Brain Nerve ; 72(6): 643-651, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32507762

RESUMEN

In this study we investigated the pronunciation error patterns in Kanji words of two patients with pure apraxia of speech. We asked the patients to read aloud 80 visually presented Kanji words, half of which were homonymous and the rest non-homonymous. The words were presented at three different intervals (2, 1.5 and 1 sec.). Case A, a 68-year-old right-handed woman, produced a variety of sound errors - substitution, distortion and omission - and some errors of pitch position. Case B, a 73-year-old right-handed woman, omitted the prolonged vowels of each word, and produced many pitch errors, including pitch position errors and flattening of pitch. Each patient had different lesions in the left precentral gyrus - Case A from the posterior bank to the middle part of the left precentral gyrus, and Case B in the anterior bank and deep white matter of the left precentral gyrus. We speculated that Case A has difficulty mainly in the articulation process and vocalization, whereas Case B's deficit lies mainly in the phonation process, which relates to control of pitch and expiration.


Asunto(s)
Apraxias , Lectura , Habla , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos
9.
Brain Dev ; 42(3): 237-247, 2020 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31862269

RESUMEN

AIMS: Current tools to evaluate Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) symptoms are not adapted to Japanese cultural and educational contexts. We aimed to develop a new tool, the Developmental Coordination Disorder Checklist (DCDC), that can be applied specifically to elementary school-aged children in Japan, by generating a list of culturally relevant items. METHODS: We designed an 18-item questionnaire that was distributed to parents and teachers of elementary school-aged children. The degree of negative influence on a child's life from their motor ability was estimated using a visual analogue scale (VAS). RESULTS: We analyzed the responses from parents/guardians (n = 1174) and teachers (n = 688) of elementary school children (age range: 75-147 months, 1st-6th grades). An exploratory analysis showed that the DCD items could be divided into three factors: fine motor, gross motor, and catch and throw. Internal consistency reliabilities of each factor and of total DCDC were sufficiently high (all coefficients of Cronbach's alpha: > 0.75). Inter-rater reliability between parent/guardian and teacher was moderate in each sub-score and total DCDC score. Parents/guardians evaluated the degree of difficulty in the fine motor factor more severely than teachers. Total DCDC score was correlated significantly with VAS value (r = 0.545), with each factor in DCDC proving useful in predicting negative impact on daily life. CONCLUSIONS: DCDC is an efficient and culturally relevant assessment tool for DCD symptoms within Japan, particularly for measuring any practical deficits in daily life activities.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Destreza Motora/diagnóstico , Psicometría/instrumentación , Psicometría/normas , Lista de Verificación , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Padres , Maestros , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
10.
J Neuropsychol ; 14(1): 135-153, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30192412

RESUMEN

Time estimation in patients with prefrontal cortex (PFC) damage is often inaccurate. The relationship between PFC and estimation of short time intervals has been examined. However, it remains unclear whether PFC damage affects estimation of longer time intervals. Here, we investigated the ability of patients and healthy subjects to verbally estimate a period of 30 min, using a method easily applied in clinical settings. In 99 patients with brain damage, we compared under and normal ranges of time in patients with PFC damage or damage to other brain areas with the chi-squared test. Subsequently, we conducted a discriminant analysis and a multiple linear regression analysis to identify specific brain areas affecting time estimation. We observed a significantly larger number of patients who overestimated 30 min in the group with bilateral PFC damage compared to patients with damage to other regions. Discriminant analysis revealed that damage of right lateral PFC and left medial PFC contributed to discrimination between the normal range and overestimation groups. Multiple linear regression analysis indicated that right lateral PFC damage strongly affected overestimation of a 30-min interval. Neuropsychological test results revealed lower general cognitive function scores and orientation scores in overestimation group. The length of estimated time and the score of delayed word recall were negatively correlated. We propose that these may require encoding, maintenance, and updating of memory and are indirectly related to contextual memory. We discuss hypotheses on contextual memory segmentation and reconstruction to clarify the mechanism of impaired time overestimation in PFC-damaged patients.


Asunto(s)
Daño Encefálico Crónico/fisiopatología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Cognición , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Memoria , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estudios Retrospectivos
11.
Front Neurosci ; 13: 406, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31139041

RESUMEN

Previous research has reported that different coping types (active or passive) are required depending on the stress-inducing task. The aim of this study was to examine the autonomic nervous response during speech tasks that require active coping, by using Lorenz plot analysis. Thirty-one university students participated in this study (M = 21.03 years, SD = 2.27). This study included 3 phases: (1) resting phase, (2) silent reading phase, and (3) reading aloud phase. Autonomic nervous system responses were recorded in each phase. We asked participants to evaluate their subjective states (arousal, valence, and mood) after the silent reading phase and the reading aloud phase. We observed that the cardiac sympathetic index (CSI) for the sympathetic nervous response was significantly higher during the reading aloud phase than during the silent reading phase. In contrast, the cardiac vagal index (CVI) for the parasympathetic nervous response was significantly higher during the reading aloud phase than during the resting phase. There were no significant differences between the resting phase and the silent reading phase in both cardiac sympathetic and CVIs. We also observed that the degree of arousal was significantly higher after the reading aloud phase than after the silent reading phase. Our findings indicate that the psychological load during silent reading is ineffective for activating the sympathetic nervous system. The sympathetic nervous response was activated in the reading aloud phase. Also, the parasympathetic nervous response in the reading aloud phase was activated compared with the resting phase. Reading aloud is necessary to adequately activate the parasympathetic nervous system by requiring participants to respire (i.e., expiration) more than during resting and silent reading tasks. The increase in the CVI likely stems from activating the parasympathetic nervous system during expiration. Although the speech task required participants to perform active coping, it was designed to activate both the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems during expiration.

12.
No To Hattatsu ; 40(5): 363-9, 2008 Sep.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18807883

RESUMEN

We developed a rapid word reading test for examining the phonological processing ability of Japanese children. We prepared two versions of the test, version A and B. Each test has word and non-word tasks. Twenty-two healthy boys of third grade in primary schools participated in this validation study. For criterion related validity, we performed the serial Hiragana reading test, the sentence reading test, Raven's coloured progressive matrices (RCPM), the Token test for children, the Kana word dictation test, the standardized comprehension test of abstract words (SCTAW), and Trail Circle test. The reading times of the newly developed test correlated moderately or highly with those of the serial Hiragana reading test and the sentence reading test. However, the scores of the other tests (RCPM, Token test for children, Kana word dictation test, SCTAW, Trail Circle test) did not correlated with the reading time of the rapid word reading test. Test-retest reliabilities in the word tasks were more than moderate: 0.52 and 0.76 in versions A and B, while those in the non-word tasks were high: 0.91 and 0.88 in versions A and B. The correlation coefficient between versions A and B was 0.7 for the word tasks and 0.92 for the non-word tasks. This study showed that the rapid word reading test has substantial validity and reliability for testing the phonological processing ability of Japanese children. In addition, the non-word tasks were more suitable for selectively examining the speed of the grapheme to phoneme conversion process.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Lectura , Niño , Humanos , Masculino
13.
J Voice ; 32(5): 585-591, 2018 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28802787

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The aim of this study was to evaluate speech in patients with adductor spasmodic dysphonia (ADSD) by perceptual evaluations and acoustic measures, and to examine the reliability and validity of these measures. METHODS: Twenty-four patients with ADSD and 24 healthy volunteers were included in the study. Speech materials consisted of three sentences constructed from serial voiced syllables to elicit abductor voice breaks. Three otolaryngologists rated the degree of voice symptoms using a visual analog scale (VAS). VAS sheets with five 100-mm horizontal lines were given to each rater. The ends of the lines were labeled normal vs severe, and the five lines were labeled as overall severity of each of the four speech symptoms (strangulation, interruption, tremor and strained speech). Nine words were selected for acoustic analysis, and abnormal acoustic events were classified into one of the three categories. To evaluate the intra- and inter-rater and intermeasurer reliabilities of the VAS scores or acoustic measures, Pearson r correlations were calculated. To examine the validity of perceptual evaluations and acoustic measures, the sensitivity and the specificity were calculated. RESULTS: Pearson r correlation coefficients for overall severity showed the highest intra- and inter-rater reliabilities. For acoustic events, the intrameasurer reliabilities were r = .645 (frequency shifts), r = .969 (aperiodic segments), and r = 1.0 (phonation breaks), and the intermeasurer reliability ranged from r = .102 to r = 1.0. Perceptual evaluation showed high sensitivity (91.7%) and specificity (100%), whereas acoustic analysis showed low sensitivity (70.8%) and high specificity (100%). CONCLUSION: Both perceptual evaluation and acoustic measures alone were found likely to overlook patients with true ADSD.


Asunto(s)
Acústica , Disfonía/diagnóstico , Juicio , Acústica del Lenguaje , Percepción del Habla , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Calidad de la Voz , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Disfonía/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
14.
J Neurol Sci ; 260(1-2): 175-82, 2007 Sep 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17553526

RESUMEN

The incidence of dementia and risk factors has not been fully investigated in Japan. Following a prevalence study in 1998, we investigated the incidence and associated factors in the same population in 2003 and 2005. Randomly selected 771 residents in Tajiri were targeted. The final participants included 204 (65.2%) healthy older adults (Clinical Dementia Rating, CDR 0) and 335 (73.1%) people with questionable dementia (CDR 0.5). We analyzed the incidence of dementia and dementing diseases, and possible risk factors. The risk factors included demographics, lifestyle-related factors, vascular risk factors, cognitive functions, and MRI findings. Overall, 3.9% of the CDR 0 and 37.0% of the CDR 0.5 participants developed dementia during the 5-year period, whereas 40.2% of the CDR 0.5 participants developed dementia during the 7-year period. Older adults had a higher incidence. Higher CDR Box scores had a higher incidence. Of the dementing diseases, 60.8% of participants developed Alzheimer' disease (AD), followed by vascular dementia (VaD), 17.9%. Logistic regression analyses showed that age, MMSE, cognitive functions such as recent memory, and generalized atrophy were significant predictors of progression to AD. Similarly, predictive factors for progression to VaD were age, MMSE, cognitive functions such as frontal function, and white matter lesions and cerebrovascular diseases. A comprehensive system including CDR, cognitive tests, and MRI, is recommended in community-based health policy planning.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/epidemiología , Demencia/epidemiología , Distribución por Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Trastornos Cerebrovasculares/diagnóstico , Trastornos Cerebrovasculares/epidemiología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Estudios de Cohortes , Comorbilidad , Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia Vascular/diagnóstico , Demencia Vascular/epidemiología , Demografía , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Japón/epidemiología , Estilo de Vida , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/estadística & datos numéricos , Masculino , Tamizaje Masivo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Factores de Riesgo
15.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol ; 20(1): 34-40, 2007 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17341769

RESUMEN

Although confabulations and delusions are observed in Alzheimer's disease, the relationship between the 2 has not been fully investigated. This study involved 50 patients with Alzheimer's disease and 10 healthy participants. After the patients were divided into delusional and nondelusional groups, confabulations and cognitive function were assessed. No confabulations appeared in the healthy participants, and only patients with Alzheimer's disease showed confabulations. The delusional group produced more confabulations on episodic subjects than on semantic subjects. There was a correlation between cognitive impairment and confabulations in semantic memory. These findings suggest that different mechanisms are involved in confabulations between semantic and episodic memories.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Amnesia/diagnóstico , Deluciones/diagnóstico , Recuerdo Mental , Represión Psicológica , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Amnesia/psicología , Deluciones/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos Mentales/diagnóstico , Trastornos Mentales/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Valores de Referencia
16.
Auton Neurosci ; 205: 72-76, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28529060

RESUMEN

We examined the relationship between anxiety sensitivity (AS) and autonomic nervous system responses (ANS) during the cold pressor test (CPT). Seventy-four university students participated and were divided into low-AS (M=9.06, SD=3.97) and high-AS groups (M=28.68, SD=6.63) based on AS Index scores (n's=36 and 38, respectively). The study included three phases: Rest, CPT, and Recovery. We measured the psychological variables (fear of pain and subjective pain) at pre- and post-CPT. ANS response data were collected during each phase. Fear of pain was experienced more strongly in the high-AS group (M=4.74, SD=3.25) relative to the low-AS group (M=2.72, SD=2.31), and subjective pain was also stronger in the high-AS group (M=3.08, SD=1.91) relative to the low-AS group (M=2.47, SD=1.00) in post-CPT. While parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) responses did not differ between the two groups during the CPT, the high AS-group demonstrated lower PNS activity during the Recovery phase. The high-AS group reported significantly more anticipatory fear and pain prior to the CPT, which appeared to aggravate subjective pain experiences. Furthermore, for individuals with anxiety sensitivity, ANS reactivity may be the mechanism underlying the relationship between negative affect and subjective pain.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Percepción del Dolor , Dolor/fisiopatología , Dolor/psicología , Anticipación Psicológica , Frío , Electrocardiografía , Miedo , Femenino , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Humanos , Masculino , Dimensión del Dolor , Sistema Nervioso Parasimpático/fisiopatología , Pruebas de Personalidad , Adulto Joven
17.
Nihon Rinsho ; 69 Suppl 8: 398-402, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22787821
18.
J Clin Neurosci ; 26: 150-2, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26597609

RESUMEN

The present study describes a Japanese patient with pure agraphia displaying differential disturbances in processing Kanji (morphogram) and Kana (syllabogram) letters after an infarction in the middle and superior portions of the left precentral gyrus. Kana errors reflected the patient's difficulty with retrieving both motor and visual letter images, whereas Kanji errors included partial letter stroke omissions or additions. This present case suggests that differences in writing disturbances between Kana and Kanji letters are caused by a differential dependency on letter motor images.


Asunto(s)
Agrafia/etiología , Infarto Encefálico/complicaciones , Infarto Encefálico/patología , Lóbulo Frontal/patología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Accidente Cerebrovascular/patología
19.
Arch Neurol ; 59(7): 1109-14, 2002 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12117358

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Vascular dementia (VaD) has been considered to be more prevalent than Alzheimer disease in Japan. However, this might be the result of overdiagnosis stemming from some problematic diagnosis of VaD or of the frequent use of magnetic resonance imaging to detect cerebrovascular disease in older adults. OBJECTIVES: We investigated the prevalence of dementia and the ratios of dementing diseases. The effects of different criteria for VaD (DSM-IV, Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers [ADDTC], and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and the Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l'Enseignement en Neurosciences [NINDS-AIREN]) were considered. Hippocampal atrophy and vascular contribution to dementia were evaluated using magnetic resonance imaging findings. METHODS: We targeted all residents 65 years and older (n = 3207) in Tajiri, Japan, and examined 1654 (participant group 1). Of these, 564 (participant group 2) were randomly selected, and 497 underwent magnetic resonance imaging and diagnosis of dementing diseases. RESULTS: We found the overall prevalence of dementia to be 8.5% (141/1654) in participant group 1. Of these, 21 (14.9%) had a history of stroke. Of the 113 participants who had a history of stroke independent of dementia, 18.6% (21/113) were demented. For participant group 2 (n = 497), 32 were demented. The ratio among the dementia for probable VaD based on the NINDS-AIREN criteria was 18.8% (6/32), whereas that for ischemic vascular dementia was 31.3% (10/32) according to the ADDTC criteria. CONCLUSION: We confirmed the overall prevalence of dementia in adults 65 years and older to be 8.5%. We found that VaD was not a common disorder according to the NINDS-AIREN criteria. Rather, the condition of possible Alzheimer disease with cerebrovascular disease was more common.


Asunto(s)
Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia/epidemiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/epidemiología , Demencia Vascular/diagnóstico , Demencia Vascular/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Japón/epidemiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Prevalencia
20.
No To Shinkei ; 55(3): 257-63, 2003 Mar.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12728508

RESUMEN

After the successful treatment of a hypothalamic germinoma, a 31-year-old right-handed male developed a difficulty in memory and admitted to our department for detailed evaluation. Neuropsychologically he showed no personality change, confabulation, misidentification, delusion or disorientation to physical time. Neither aphasia, dementia or frontal lobe dysfunction was found. However, he showed a moderate degree of anterograde amnesia and a retrograde amnesia for the last 5 years judged by a public events test. Moreover, his temporal markings of a correctly remembered event in the post high school days shifted strongly toward the high school days. This temporal location abnormality was largely limited to the events from the days of his graduation from the high school to the period 5 years prior to the present incident. He felt personal events happened during this period as if they had occurred in the high school days, although he well knew that they actually happened much later. At the same time he felt that memories of the high school days were abnormally vivid and recent. Most interestingly, he was found to have been experiencing a funny feeling that he was living in the high school days and the present at the same time. The feeling occurred either simultaneously or alternatively. In the latter instance, he would suddenly start behaving as if he were a high school student. He would start urging his wife to go to the school together immediately. This behavior would be over in several minutes. His consciousness was clear in these instances and there was no amnesia for these periods. A brain MRI with gadolinium enhancement showed a lesion extending in the hypothalamus, anterior thalamus, basal forebrain and midbrain bilaterally. After eight months this abnormality of temporal sensation disappeared. However, tendency to make the date of public event nearer to the high school period persisted. We hypothesize that impaired temporal estimation for an event recalled from the retrograde memory store and mis-arousal of familiarity evoked at the time of spontaneous recollection are responsible for double feeling of time of an experience. These phenomenon might be attributable to the partial destruction of the Papez' and the basolateral limbic circuits.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Anterógrada , Amnesia Retrógrada , Germinoma/psicología , Neoplasias Hipotalámicas/psicología , Recuerdo Mental , Adulto , Conducta , Germinoma/diagnóstico , Humanos , Neoplasias Hipotalámicas/diagnóstico , Hipotálamo Anterior/patología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda