Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 26
Filtrar
1.
Conscious Cogn ; 22(3): 677-83, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23681207

RESUMO

The ability to imagine hypothetical events in one's personal future is thought to involve a number of constituent cognitive processes. We investigated the extent to which individual differences in working memory capacity contribute to facets of episodic future thought. College students completed simple and complex measures of working memory and were cued to recall autobiographical memories and imagine future autobiographical events consisting of varying levels of specificity (i.e., ranging from generic to increasingly specific and detailed events). Consistent with previous findings, future thought was related to analogous measures of autobiographical memory, likely reflecting overlapping cognitive factors supporting both past and future thought. Additionally, after controlling for autobiographical memory, residual working memory variance independently predicted future episodic specificity. We suggest that when imagining future events, working memory contributes to the construction of a single, coherent, future event depiction, but not to the retrieval or elaboration of event details.


Assuntos
Imaginação/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Memória Episódica , Pensamento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
2.
Anxiety Stress Coping ; 35(4): 474-487, 2022 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34550823

RESUMO

BACKGROUND & OBJECTIVES: Emotion regulation is an integral component of multiple physical and psychological functions, and problems with emotion regulation are thought to be involved with the development of psychological disorders. Most laboratory research on emotion regulation has been limited to investigating short-term impact during a single session. DESIGN & METHODS: In the current study, we investigated the potential delayed consequences of the short-term use of two emotion regulation strategies: mindfulness and suppression. To do so, a two-session design was implemented, with 56 undergraduate participants viewing images under various regulation instructions at Time 1, returning one week later for a recognition memory test for these images at Time 2. Images were rated for valence and arousal at both time points. RESULTS: We hypothesized that images encoded mindfully would elicit lower levels of anxiety upon re-exposure, while suppression would increase levels of anxiety. Results did not support original hypotheses: although participants reported being calmer and happier at re-exposure, this effect did not vary by strategy. High-anxiety participants reported higher arousal levels regardless of Instruction condition or Time Point. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest that short-term use of emotion regulation does not impact feelings of calmness long-term, when exposed to anxiety-provoking stimuli.


Assuntos
Regulação Emocional , Atenção Plena , Ansiedade/psicologia , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Humanos , Atenção Plena/métodos
3.
Future Healthc J ; 9(1): 34-40, 2022 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35372780

RESUMO

The third industrial revolution has radically impacted the transformation of hospitals. Through the adoption of key digital technologies, hospitals have become more accessible, flexible, organised, responsive and able to deliver more personalised care. The digitalisation of patient health records, one of the most remarkable achievements to date in healthcare management, has enabled new opportunities, including the idea of hospitals evolving to become artificially intelligent. In parallel, the adoption of electronic and mobile internet technologies in hospitals has introduced new structural concepts, seeing a variety of terms blossom such as 'smart', 'intelligent', 'green' and 'liquid'. Now in the early fourth industrial revolution, driven by AI and internet-of-things technologies, this article unveils a new concept adapted to the upcoming era.

4.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 75(1): 80-84, 2020 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31504914

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The main objective of this study was to determine whether there are age differences in positive and negative repetitive thought (ie, rumination). METHOD: Young adults (ages 19-39; n = 114) and older adults (ages 60-85; n = 88) completed measures of negative and positive rumination. Bayesian analyses were used to determine whether age differences were present for both negative (young > old) and positive (old > young) rumination. RESULTS: There was extremely strong evidence for age differences in negative rumination, with lower scores in older adults. In contrast, the evidence was in favor of the null hypothesis for positive rumination. DISCUSSION: Age-related positivity is better characterized as decreased dwelling on the meaning of negative moods, rather than increased attention to positive ones.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Regulação Emocional/fisiologia , Ruminação Cognitiva/fisiologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Teorema de Bayes , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
5.
Neuroimage ; 42(4): 1577-86, 2008 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18634891

RESUMO

A long-standing assumption in the cognitive aging literature is that performance on working memory (WM) tasks involving serial recall is relatively unaffected by aging, whereas tasks that require the rearrangement of items prior to recall are more age-sensitive. Previous neuroimaging studies of WM have found age-related increases in neural activity in frontoparietal brain regions during simple maintenance tasks, but few have examined whether there are age-related differences that are specific to rearranging WM items. In the current study, older and younger adults' brain activity was monitored using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) as they performed WM tasks involving either maintenance or manipulation (letter-number sequencing). The paradigm was developed so that performance was equivalent across age groups in both tasks, and the manipulation condition was not more difficult than the maintenance condition. In younger adults, manipulation-related increases in activation occurred within a very focal set of regions within the canonical brain WM network, including left posterior prefrontal cortex and bilateral inferior parietal cortex. In contrast, older adults showed a much wider extent of manipulation-related activation within this WM network, with significantly increased activity relative to younger adults found within bilateral PFC. The results suggest that activation and age-differences in lateral PFC engagement during WM manipulation conditions may reflect strategy use and controlled processing demands rather than reflect the act of manipulation per se.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino
6.
Psychol Aging ; 23(1): 2-12, 2008 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18361649

RESUMO

The current study examines how the instructions given during picture viewing impact age differences in incidental emotional memory. Previous research has suggested that older adults' memory may be better when they make emotional rather than perceptual evaluations of stimuli and that their memory may show a positivity bias in tasks with open-ended viewing instructions. Across two experiments, participants viewing photographs either received open-ended instructions or were asked to make emotionally focused (Experiment 1) or perceptually focused (Experiment 2) evaluations. Emotional evaluations had no impact on older adults' memory, whereas perceptual evaluations reduced older adults' recall of emotional, but not of neutral, pictures. Evidence for the positivity effect was sporadic and was not easier to detect with open-ended viewing instructions. These results suggest that older adults' memory is best when the material to be remembered is emotionally evocative and they are allowed to process it as such.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Atenção , Emoções , Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Julgamento , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
7.
Psychol Aging ; 23(3): 634-45, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18808252

RESUMO

It has been hypothesized that older adults are especially susceptible to proactive interference (PI) and that this may contribute to age differences in working memory performance. In young adults, individual differences in PI affect both working memory and reasoning ability, but the relations between PI, working memory, and reasoning in older adults have not been examined. In the current study, young, old, and very old adults performed a modified operation span task that induced several cycles of PI buildup and release as well as two tests of abstract reasoning ability. Age differences in working memory scores increased as PI built up, consistent with the hypothesis that older adults are more susceptible to PI, but both young and older adults showed complete release from PI. Young adults' reasoning ability was best predicted by working memory performance under high PI conditions, replicating M. Bunting (2006). In contrast, older adults' reasoning ability was best predicted by their working memory performance under low PI conditions, thereby raising questions regarding the general role of susceptibility to PI in differences in higher cognitive function among older adults.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Cognição , Memória , Inibição Proativa , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Distribuição por Idade , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Atenção , Humanos , Individualidade , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Probabilidade , Resolução de Problemas , Tempo de Reação , Aprendizagem Seriada
8.
Motiv Emot ; 42(3): 446-457, 2018 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29983474

RESUMO

The current study sought to better understand the utility of two strategies - perspective-taking and facial mimicry - proposed to increase empathic responding. Thirty-seven female participants were presented an interpersonal situation (a betrayal) that would elicit the use of empathic responding to achieve conflict resolution between friends. Each participant was given instructions to partake in either perspective-taking, facial mimicry, or to remain neutral (control condition). The results demonstrated that individuals who engaged in perspective-taking reported significantly higher state empathy than the control condition, but there was no significant difference in state empathy between the mimicry and control condition. Also, those who engaged in either strategy reported significantly higher self-other overlap relative to those not instructed to engage in a particular strategy. Importantly, self-other overlap mediated the association between the instructional sets and state empathy. Both strategies are arguably means of enhancing interpersonal understanding.

9.
J Pediatr ; 150(5): 461-6, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17452216

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Children with type 1 diabetes (T1DM) are at increased risk for celiac disease (CD); however, the benefits of screening for IgA tissue transglutaminase autoantibodies (TG), a marker for CD, are unclear. STUDY DESIGN: We compared 71 screening-identified TG+ with 63 matched TG- children with TIDM. Growth, bone density, and diabetes control measures were obtained. RESULTS: The group was 10 +/- 3 years of age, 46% male, with TIDM for 4 +/- 3 years. Z scores for weight (0.3 +/- 1 vs 0.7 +/- 0.8, P = .024), body mass index (BMI) (0.3 +/- 0.9 vs 0.8 +/- -0.8, P = .005), and midarm circumference (0.3 +/- 1.1 vs 0.6 +/- 0.9, P = .031) were lower in the TG+ group. Bone mineral density and diabetes control measures were similar. When limiting the analysis to the 35 TG+ subjects with biopsy changes of CD, the BMI Z score was lower than the control group (0.4 +/- 0.9 vs 0.7 +/- 0.7, P = .05). CONCLUSIONS: In children with TIDM, screening-identified evidence of CD is associated with altered body composition, but not bone mineral density or diabetes control. Further study is needed to determine the benefit of early diagnosis and treatment of CD in TIDM children.


Assuntos
Autoanticorpos/sangue , Doença Celíaca/sangue , Doença Celíaca/imunologia , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/sangue , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/complicações , Autoimunidade , Doença Celíaca/complicações , Doença Celíaca/diagnóstico , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transglutaminases/imunologia
10.
Psychol Aging ; 22(1): 75-83, 2007 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17385985

RESUMO

The authors report 2 experiments in which they examined age differences in working memory tasks involving complex item manipulation (i.e., letter-number sequencing). In Experiment 1, age differences on tasks involving item manipulation were not greater than age differences on tasks requiring recall of items in the order in which they appeared, suggesting that older adults do not have difficulty with item manipulation per se. In Experiment 2, slower presentation rates increased age differences in item manipulation spans, although age differences at the fastest rate may be attributed to differences in strategy use. In both experiments, age differences were largest when participants were most likely to be remembering familiar sequences, suggesting that older adults may have difficulties dampening the representations of such sequences once they are activated.


Assuntos
Memória , Tempo de Reação , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Feminino , Nível de Saúde , Humanos , Masculino , Prática Psicológica
11.
J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr ; 45(3): 293-300, 2007 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17873740

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Gliadin proteins play a key role in the pathogenesis of celiac disease; however, as a screen for celiac disease, anti-gliadin antibody testing has been replaced by the more sensitive and specific serological assays for transglutaminase autoantibodies (TGAA). A new generation of anti-gliadin antibody assays has been developed to detect synthetic, deamidated homologous gliadin peptides (DGP) with high sensitivity and specificity. METHODS: Sera were collected prospectively from children with an increased risk for celiac disease as part of an ongoing study at Denver, and studied for the development of celiac autoimmunity. We investigated the high-performance DGP antibody assay in 50 TGAA-positive children both before the development of celiac autoimmunity and following the institution of a gluten-free diet to determine the relationship of DGP antibodies to TGAA. TGAA were measured by an in-house radioassay. RESULTS: DGP antibodies and TGAA parallel each other over the period of years children were studied. DGP antibodies resolved sooner than TGAA in subjects on a gluten-free diet. DGP antibodies appeared earlier than TGAA in 9 children. CONCLUSIONS: Measuring DGP antibodies may be more useful than TGAA in monitoring children on a gluten-free diet. DGP antibodies can precede the appearance of TGAA in some at-risk children.


Assuntos
Autoanticorpos , Doença Celíaca/diagnóstico , Gliadina/administração & dosagem , Gliadina/imunologia , Transglutaminases/imunologia , Adolescente , Autoanticorpos/análise , Doença Celíaca/sangue , Doença Celíaca/genética , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Predisposição Genética para Doença , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Programas de Rastreamento , Fragmentos de Peptídeos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Fatores de Risco , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
13.
Diabetes ; 54(8): 2482-6, 2005 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16046318

RESUMO

Approximately 50% of the genetic risk for type 1 diabetes is attributable to the HLA region. We evaluated associations between candidate genes outside the HLA region-INS, cytotoxic T-lymphocyte-associated antigen (CTLA)-4, interleukin (IL)-4, IL-4R, and IL-13 and islet autoimmunity among children participating in the Diabetes Autoimmunity Study in the Young (DAISY). Children with persistent islet autoantibody positivity (n = 102, 38 of whom have already developed diabetes) and control subjects (n = 198) were genotyped for single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the candidate genes. The INS-23Hph1 polymorphism was significantly associated with both type 1 diabetes (OR = 0.30; 95% CI 0.13-0.69) and persistent islet autoimmunity but in the latter, only in children with the HLA-DR3/4 genotype (0.40; 0.18-0.89). CTLA-4 promoter SNP was significantly associated with type 1 diabetes (3.52; 1.22-10.17) but not with persistent islet autoimmunity. Several SNPs in the IL-4 regulatory pathway appeared to have a predisposing effect for type 1 diabetes. Associations were found between both IL-4R haplotypes and IL-4-IL-13 haplotypes and persistent islet autoimmunity and type 1 diabetes. This study confirms the association between the INS and CTLA-4 loci and type 1 diabetes. Genes involved in the IL-4 regulatory pathway (IL-4, IL-4R, IL-13) may confer susceptibility or protection to type 1 diabetes depending on individual SNPs or specific haplotypes.


Assuntos
Autoimunidade/genética , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/imunologia , Ilhotas Pancreáticas/imunologia , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único/genética , Adolescente , Antígenos CD , Antígenos de Diferenciação/genética , Antígeno CTLA-4 , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Genótipo , Haplótipos , Humanos , Lactente , Insulina/genética , Interleucina-13/genética , Interleucina-4/genética , Masculino , Receptores de Interleucina-4/genética
14.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 61(6): P366-8, 2006 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17114307

RESUMO

We examined whether the type of response used to report items recalled from short-term memory affects the age difference in verbal and spatial memory spans. Younger and older adults viewed either a series of letters or a series of locations in a grid, and then they reported their memory for the items either vocally or by using a touch screen. Overall, age differences were larger for spatial memory spans than for verbal memory spans, replicating previous results. Changing the response modality affected only older adults' verbal spans, which were approximately one item higher with a vocal response than with a manual response. This resulted in a smaller age difference for verbal items reported vocally than for any other condition. The results can best be explained by age-related difficulties in both spatial processing and in dealing with stimulus-response incongruity.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Tato , Comportamento Verbal , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Percepção Espacial
15.
Diabetes Care ; 28(2): 296-300, 2005 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15677782

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Families of children diagnosed with type 1 diabetes require counseling concerning type 1 diabetes risk in nondiabetic siblings and parents. No U.S. population-specific life-table risk estimates are currently available for parents, and those for siblings (2-6% by age 20 years) are based on family studies completed before 1987. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: We analyzed family histories of 1,586 patients in Colorado with type 1 diabetes (83% non-Hispanic white, 10% Hispanic, and 7% other) diagnosed before 16 years of age and interviewed during 1999-2002. Families of probands with type 2, undetermined, or secondary diabetes (n = 53) or those with incomplete data (n = 137) were excluded. The median age at onset of the proband was 7.1 years and the median diabetes duration 3.5 years. Cumulative risk estimates were calculated using survival analysis for 2,081 full siblings and 3,016 biological parents. RESULTS: In siblings, the overall risk of type 1 diabetes by age 20 years was 4.4%, but it was significantly (P < 0.0001) higher in siblings of probands diagnosed under age 7 years than in those diagnosed later. In parents, the overall risk by age 40 years was 2.6% and higher in fathers (3.6%) than in mothers (1.7%) of probands (P < 0.001). Similar to siblings, the risk was also higher (P = 0.006) in parents of probands diagnosed <7 years of age than in those diagnosed later. CONCLUSIONS: Current risks of type 1 diabetes in Colorado siblings and parents of type 1 diabetic probands are higher than in the 1982 Pittsburgh study but similar to contemporary European rates. Recurrence risk of type 1 diabetes is significantly higher in first-degree relatives of probands diagnosed at a young age.


Assuntos
Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/epidemiologia , Adulto , Idade de Início , Autoanticorpos/sangue , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Colorado/epidemiologia , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/genética , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/imunologia , Pai , Feminino , Humanos , Incidência , Ilhotas Pancreáticas/imunologia , Masculino , Mães , Análise Multivariada , Fatores de Risco , Irmãos
16.
JAMA ; 293(19): 2343-51, 2005 May 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15900004

RESUMO

CONTEXT: While gluten ingestion is responsible for the signs and symptoms of celiac disease, it is not known what factors are associated with initial appearance of the disease. OBJECTIVE: To examine whether the timing of gluten exposure in the infant diet was associated with the development of celiac disease autoimmunity (CDA). DESIGN, SETTING, AND PATIENTS: Prospective observational study conducted in Denver, Colo, from 1994-2004 of 1560 children at increased risk for celiac disease or type 1 diabetes, as defined by possession of either HLA-DR3 or DR4 alleles, or having a first-degree relative with type 1 diabetes. The mean follow-up was 4.8 years. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: Risk of CDA defined as being positive for tissue transglutaminase (tTG) autoantibody on 2 or more consecutive visits or being positive for tTG once and having a positive small bowel biopsy for celiac disease, by timing of introduction of gluten-containing foods into the diet. RESULTS: Fifty-one children developed CDA. Findings adjusted for HLA-DR3 status indicated that children exposed to foods containing wheat, barley, or rye (gluten-containing foods) in the first 3 months of life (3 [6%] CDA positive vs 40 [3%] CDA negative) had a 5-fold increased risk of CDA compared with children exposed to gluten-containing foods at 4 to 6 months (12 [23%] CDA positive vs 574 [38%] CDA negative) (hazard ratio [HR], 5.17; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.44-18.57). Children not exposed to gluten until the seventh month or later (36 [71%] CDA positive vs 895 [59%] CDA negative) had a marginally increased risk of CDA compared with those exposed at 4 to 6 months (HR, 1.87; 95% CI, 0.97-3.60). After restricting our case group to only the 25 CDA-positive children who had biopsy-diagnosed celiac disease, initial exposure to wheat, barley, or rye in the first 3 months (3 [12%] CDA positive vs 40 [3%] CDA negative) or in the seventh month or later (19 [76%] CDA positive vs 912 [59%] CDA negative) significantly increased risk of CDA compared with exposure at 4 to 6 months (3 [12%] CDA positive vs 583 [38%] CDA negative) (HR, 22.97; 95% CI, 4.55-115.93; P = .001; and HR, 3.98; 95% CI, 1.18-13.46; P = .04, respectively). CONCLUSION: Timing of introduction of gluten into the infant diet is associated with the appearance of CDA in children at increased risk for the disease.


Assuntos
Doença Celíaca/imunologia , Dieta , Grão Comestível , Glutens/administração & dosagem , Autoanticorpos , Autoimunidade , Biópsia , Doença Celíaca/diagnóstico , Doença Celíaca/epidemiologia , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente , Intestino Delgado/patologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Fatores de Tempo , Transglutaminases/imunologia
17.
Psychol Aging ; 28(3): 853-63, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23421322

RESUMO

Two experiments examined the impact of encoding conditions and information content in memory for positive, neutral, and negative pictures. We examined the hypotheses that the positivity effect in memory (i.e., a bias in favor of positive or against negative information in later life) would be reduced when (a) pictures were viewed under structured as opposed to unstructured conditions, and (b) contained social as opposed to nonsocial content. Both experiments found that the positivity effect observed with nonsocial stimuli was absent with social stimuli. In addition, little evidence was obtained that encoding conditions affected the strength of the positivity effect. We argue that some types of social stimuli may engage different types of processing than nonsocial stimuli, perhaps encouraging self-referential processing that engages attention and supports memory. This processing may then conflict with the goal-driven, top-down processing that is hypothesized to drive the positivity effect. Thus, our results identify further boundary conditions associated with the positivity effect in memory, arguing that stimulus factors as well as situational goals may affect its occurrence. Further research awaits to determine if this effect is specific to all social stimuli or specific subsets.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22292431

RESUMO

Recent research suggests that affective and motivational processes can influence age differences in memory. In the current study, we examine the impact of both natural and induced mood state on age differences in false recall. Older and younger adults performed a version of the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM; Roediger & McDermott, 1995 , Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803) false memory paradigm in either their natural mood state or after a positive or negative mood induction. Results indicated that, after accounting for age differences in basic cognitive function, age-related differences in positive mood during the testing session were related to increased false recall in older adults. Inducing older adults into a positive mood also exacerbated age differences in false memory. In contrast, veridical recall did not appear to be systematically influenced by mood. Together, these results suggest that positive mood states can impact older adults' information processing and potentially increase underlying cognitive age differences.


Assuntos
Afeto , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Ilusões , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Repressão Psicológica , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22059441

RESUMO

Normative age differences in memory have typically been attributed to declines in basic cognitive and cortical mechanisms. The present study examined the degree to which dominant everyday affect might also be associated with age-related memory errors using the misinformation paradigm. Younger and older adults viewed a positive and a negative event, and then were exposed to misinformation about each event. Older adults exhibited a higher likelihood than young adults of falsely identifying misinformation as having occurred in the events. Consistent with expectations, strength of the misinformation effect was positively associated with dominant mood, and controlling for mood eliminated any age effects. Also, motivation to engage in complex cognitive activity was negatively associated with susceptibility to misinformation, and susceptibility was stronger for negative than for positive events. We argue that motivational processes underlie all of the observed effects, and that such processes are useful in understanding age differences in memory performance.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Motivação/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
20.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 67(3): 299-308, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21926400

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: We investigated how fluctuations and linear changes in health and cognitive resources influence the motivation to engage in complex cognitive activity and the extent to which motivation mediated the relationship between changing resources and cognitively demanding activities. METHOD: Longitudinal data from 332 adults aged 20-85 years were examined. Motivation was assessed using a composite of Need for Cognition and Personal Need for Structure and additional measures of health, sensory functioning, cognitive ability, and self-reported activity engagement. RESULTS: Multilevel modeling revealed that age-typical changes in health, sensory functions, and ability were associated with changes in motivation, with the impact of declining health on motivation being particularly strong in older adulthood. Changes in motivation, in turn, predicted involvement in cognitive and social activities as well as changes in cognitive ability. Finally, motivation was observed to partially mediate the relationship between changes in resources and cognitively demanding activities. DISCUSSION: Our results suggest that motivation may play an important role in determining the course of cognitive change and involvement in cognitively demanding everyday activities in adulthood.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Nível de Saúde , Motivação , Ajustamento Social , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Atitude , Cognição , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Psicológicos , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa